Review Open Access
Copyright ©The Author(s) 2025. Published by Baishideng Publishing Group Inc. All rights reserved.
World J Psychiatry. Aug 19, 2025; 15(8): 107791
Published online Aug 19, 2025. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v15.i8.107791
Startle response and its prepulse modification in health and under different psychopathologies: Could we find any specific patterns?
Zinaida I Storozheva, Laboratory of Neurophysiology and Neurochemistry, Department of Neuropsychopharmacology, Federal Research Center for Innovator and Emerging Biomedical and Pharmaceutical Technologies, Moscow 125315, Russia
ORCID number: Zinaida I Storozheva (0000-0001-6280-5312).
Author contributions: Storozheva ZI designed the review framework, identified the research focus, conducted the literature search and analysis of the data, drafted the manuscript, provided critical revisions and final approval of the manuscript.
Conflict-of-interest statement: The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Open Access: This article is an open-access article that was selected by an in-house editor and fully peer-reviewed by external reviewers. It is distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution NonCommercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited and the use is non-commercial. See: https://creativecommons.org/Licenses/by-nc/4.0/
Corresponding author: Zinaida I Storozheva, Senior Scientist, Laboratory of Neurophysiology and Neurochemistry, Department of Neuropsychopharmacology, Federal Research Center for Innovator and Emerging Biomedical and Pharmaceutical Technologies, Baltiiskaya, 8, Moscow 125315, Russia. storozheva_zi@mail.ru
Received: April 2, 2025
Revised: May 2, 2025
Accepted: July 3, 2025
Published online: August 19, 2025
Processing time: 132 Days and 0.2 Hours

Abstract

The startle response (SR) is a generalized defensive response elicited by the presentation of a sudden intense stimulus. The presentation of a less intense signal (prepulse) before the central stimulus (pulse) affects the amplitude and latency of SR differently depending on the prepulse lead interval. The most studied form of such changes is prepulse inhibition (PPI), i.e. a decrease in SR amplitude at lead intervals of 50-500 ms. Prepulse facilitation, i.e. an increase in SR amplitude, can also be observed at lead intervals of 2000-4500 ms. The PPI deficiency has been found in a variety of psychopathologies and it has been suggested that it is a transdiagnostic phenomenon. However, some data from the literature support the existence of specific different nosologies, such as neurophysiological, neurochemical and molecular mechanisms that cause PPI lowering and affect prepulse facilitation of SR. This review provides a comparative analysis of studies on SR prepulse modification in healthy subjects and different groups of patients with mental or neurological disorders. The results of such an analysis may help to define directions for further research to improve methods of early diagnosis and to improve the validity of translational models.

Key Words: Startle reaction; Prepulse modification; Neuropsychiatric disorders; Attention; Genes; Brain potentials

Core Tip: There are several reviews in the literature dedicated to phenomenon of prepulse modification of the startle response. In these manuscripts its deficiency in neuropsychiatric disorders regarded as transdiagnostic process. Here we review the evidence in favor of the specificity of certain prepulse modification changes for certain pathologies of pathology and consider the prospects for future research on this issue.



INTRODUCTION

Identifying the neurophysiological and behavioral processes that constitute mental health and illness at a systems level is one of the most important research tasks in the field of biological psychiatry, but the problem of how to apply the latest neurobiological research data to the diagnosis and treatment of mental disorders remains unresolved and requires further investigation. Obviously, the success of these studies requires a permanent analysis and synthesis of the data obtained in translational research, in order to determine the quality of the existing model, as well as the direction and objectives of further research to improve it, focusing on the most relevant neurobiological phenomena.

One of the most promising tools in translational studies of the neurobiological mechanisms of psychiatric disorders is the model of the startle response (SR) and prepulse modulation, exploiting an interspecies behavioral phenomenon to study mechanisms of sensory-motor gating at the stages of preattention and early attention, attention allocation at the early stages of stimulus processing, and sustained attention to incoming sensory information at the stage of development of orienting attention[1-3].

The SR is a reflexive whole-body response to sudden high-intensity stimuli. It includes orienting and defensive components aimed at immediately modifying current behavioral activity, stimulating sensory scanning of environmental stimuli, and increasing motor readiness for further specific defensive responses: Freezing, running, attacking, etc[4,5]. In rodent laboratory studies, the magnitude of the SR is usually estimated by the increase in pressure on the chamber floor as the hind legs extend (whole-body startle)[4,5]. In primates and humans, the blink component of SR is mainly recorded by the potential of the orbicularis oculi muscle [electromyography (EMG) response][6] or by the oculographic response (electrooculogram response)[4,7].

In primate studies, the most commonly used model is the acoustic SR (ASR), which is induced by tones of at least 80 decibels and a high rate of increase (no more than 10-12 ms to peak intensity). In addition to ASR, tactile SR is also used, which can be induced by airflow directed at the eye[8], electrical stimulation of the trigeminal nerve, or sharp touching of the glabella, the skin area above the bridge of the nose[9]. Visual stimulation is also used, but the intensity of the response in this case is quite low 2-20 μV compared to 50-200 μV for the acoustic response[6]. Although SR is a very conservative form of innate behavior, its amplitude and latency can be strongly modified by current environmental stimuli.

When the main intense stimulus (pulse) is preceded by a brief stimulus of moderate intensity (prepulse), changes in SR amplitude and latency are observed. The pattern of these changes depends on the length of the interval between the prepulse and the pulse [lead interval (LI)]. The most studied type of SR modification is prepulse inhibition (PPI), the phenomenon of a decrease in SR amplitude observed when the LI duration is within 30-500 ms. The PPI cannot be explained by changes in excitability within the reflex arc of the SR, as it is also observed under conditions of cross-modal stimulation[10,11].

The PPI is thought to reflect the mechanisms of sensorimotor gating that provide adaptive response patterns to multiple stimuli. It is worth noting that in the case of PPI of the SR, we are faced with the processing of successive stimuli that differ significantly in intensity and vital salience. A sudden high-intensity startle stimulus conveys information about a threatening situation and elicits a rapid defensive response that can inhibit ongoing brain activity, including the processing of a neutral prepulse. However, because the prepulse may contain information about a source of sudden threat, the existence of specific gating mechanisms can be assumed to protect the processing of the prepulse from interruption by the sensory and motor components of the SR until the processing of the prepulse and assessment of its salience are complete. The existence of ‘interruptive’ and ‘protective’ mechanisms was originally proposed by Graham[1] and subsequently confirmed by several investigators[12,13]. At the same time, optimal behavior in the defensive situation also requires a sufficient level of response to the high-intensity startle stimulus. Neuroimaging studies in rodents[14-16] have revealed some functional brain networks involved in the PPI regulation (Figure 1).

Figure 1
Figure 1 Brain structures involved in the mechanisms of the acoustic startle response and its prepulse inhibition. Black bar: Main intensive stimulus (pulse); Grey bar: Preceding weak stimulus (prepulse); Triangle: Amplitude of startle response; White wide arrow: Protection of prepulse processing; Black wide arrow: Protection of main stimulus (pulse) processing. Data obtained in humans and rodents using lesion, stimulation and neuroimaging techniques are summarized[4,5,14-17,45,48]. PPI: Prepulse inhibition; ASR: Acoustic startle response.

The first is the main arc of the SR, which includes the cochlear nuclei, the ventrolateral tegmental nucleus and the caudal pontine reticular nucleus. The activity of this main ASR arc is under the control of midbrain structures including the inferior and superior colliculi and the dorsal periaqueductal grey. The upstream brain structures form two modulatory networks, including those involved in prepulse protection, suppression of the SR and increase in the PPI (‘PPI network’), and those involved in facilitation of startle processing and reduction of PPI (‘ASR network’). The coordinated activity of these systems ensures the distribution of processing resources in a situation where stimuli of different salience are rapidly presented. Interestingly, the ‘ASR network’ predominantly involves the left parts of the cortex (frontal and prelimbic), whereas the ‘PPI network’ involves the right prelimbic cortex. Human neuroimaging studies have also revealed interhemispheric asymmetry when assessing the involvement of forebrain structures in the PPI regulation (see for a review, although this issue needs further investigation)[17].

It should be noted that within the 30-500 ms time interval in which PPI occurs, it is possible to distinguish a segment of 30-120 ms, which is the interval of pre-attentive processing of stimuli by higher brain structures, and a period of 120-500 ms, when signal processing occurs with the involvement of early attentional mechanisms[18,19].

Another much less studied type of ASR prepulse modification is prepulse facilitation (PPF), which is an increase in response amplitude observed at LI values of 2000-6000 ms[20-22]. This “long-lead” PPF reflects mechanisms of general alertness, attentional orientation, and attentional sustainability[20-22]. Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that the PPI and PPF operate in both separate and shared networks[23]. In contrast to the PPI, which provides gating and resource allocation during the processing of sensorimotor information, PPF is the result of sensorimotor integration, i.e., the summation of the response to the prepulse with the response to the pulse.

The PPI deficiency has been reported in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders by many authors[2,10,24-41] and in the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia multi-site study[42].

The PPF has been investigated to a much lesser extent, but its deficiency in patients with schizophrenia has been demonstrated in several studies[28-30,38,40,43].

In addition to schizophrenia spectrum disorders, the PPI deficiency has been observed in a variety of psychopathologies, and it has been proposed to be considered as a transdiagnostic process in the paradigm of the dimensional approach to neuropsychiatric disorders[44]. A decrease in the level of sensorimotor regulation can also be observed in clinically healthy people without psychiatric and neurological problems[45].

At the same time, many structures have been shown to influence the PPI and PPF modulation, including the frontal cortex, amygdala, hippocampus, cerebellar cortico-striato-pallido-thalamic and frontostriatal circuits[16,17,46-48]. The mechanisms of aetiology and pathogenesis of various psychopathologies are associated with specific and rather selective changes in the activity of these structures. Thus, we can try to identify some sets of changes in PPI and PPF parameters, which are quite specific for different neurological and psychiatric pathologies.

Given the lateralization of brain structures involved in the mediation and modulation of the PPI and PPF[16,17,45-48], the laterality of the observed changes in startle modulation by prepulses in different groups of patients can be considered as a measure with potential specificity. Braff et al[2] was the first to point out the utility of recording the blink component of the ASR bilaterally. Although some authors[38-40,49-53] have reported bilateral registration of the ASR, many of them have averaged the data from both eyes when comparing experimental groups. Some studies, however, allow comparison of data from the right and left eyes[38-40,49-51].

The analysis of the effects of the length of the LI in the PPI paradigm may also be promising, as the PPI at different time points may reflect either fully automatic gating mechanisms or those dependent on early attentional processes. In fact, the pattern of activation of brain structures during the PPI test has been shown to be dependent on the length of the LI[47,48].

There may also be specific alterations in the effects of selective attention to the prepulse, which have been shown to differ between schizophrenia patients and healthy individuals[52-60], that may also show specific changes in other disorders.

Furthermore, the specificity of the PPI changes in different neurological and psychiatric disorders may be determined by the activity levels of different neurotransmitter systems and polymorphic variants of genes controlling neurotransmission processes.

These aspects deserve to be analyzed, as understanding the mechanisms of neurological and psychiatric disorders, identifying potential biomarkers and developing therapeutic approaches may be facilitated by the detection of specific patterns of alterations in sensorimotor gating and integration under different pathological conditions.

Most studies of SR prepulse modification use protocols that involve acoustic (rather than tactile) stimulation and recording of the EMG response from the orbicularis oculi muscle. Such ‘standard’ protocols also use the so-called ‘uninstructed’ paradigm, which does not include additional techniques to increase voluntary attention to the stimuli.

SEARCH STRATEGY

The search was conducted in the following electronic databases: PubMed, Web of Science and the Russian database Elibrary. The database queries were not limited to a time period. All studies were retrieved from the beginning of the database until February 2025.

The main query field contained two keywords simultaneously: Startle and prepulse. In combination with the main query, the name of one of the target neuropsychiatric disorders was included, such as schizophrenia spectrum disorder, anxiety, posttraumatic stress disorder, panic disorder, Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, Parkinson’s disease, depression, bipolar disorder (BD), obsessive-compulsive disorder, Tourette’s syndrome, autism spectrum disorder, neurodegenerative disorders. The restriction ‘human’ was introduced in all cases. In addition, searches were conducted for queries containing combinations of the keywords startle, prepulse and neural circuit; startle, prepulse and electroencephalogram (EEG); startle, prepulse and genes.

ASR PREPULSE MODIFICATION IN PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES: DATA OBTAINED USING ‘STANDARD’ PROTOCOLS

In studies using such ‘standard’ protocols, reduced PPI levels have been observed in obsessive-compulsive disorder, BD, autism, anxiety, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), Huntington’s disease, and Tourette’s syndrome[44,45]. The data obtained in the studies of these neuroses are presented in Tables 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6. The results obtained in patients with different stages of neurodegenerative disorders are also presented in Table 7. Particular attention is paid to the laterality of the effect and its dependence on the length of the LI.

Table 1 Summary of studies on prepulse inhibition and prepulse facilitation of the startle response in the cohorts of participants with schizophrenia spectrum disorders, mean ± SD.
Ref.
Groups (group size)
Mean age
Side of recording
Lead interval, ms
PPI
PPF
Xue et al[25]Unmedicated Sch (26)/medicated Sch (20)/Contr (31)25.4 ± 3.1Right 30, 60, 120Sch < Contr at LI 30 and LI 120NA
Hammer et al[26]Unmedicated Sch (27)/Contr (38)27.3 ± 3.6Right 120Sch < ContrNA
Mackeprang et al[27]Unmedicated Sch (20)/Contr (20)27.0 ± 2.4Right 30, 60, 120Sch < Contr at LI 30, 60, 120NA
Ludewig et al[28]Unmedicated Sch (24)/Contr (20)23.6 ± 4.2Right 30, 60, 120, 240, 2000Sch < Contr at LI 60Sch < Contr
Ludewig et al[29]Medicated Sch (67)/Contr (44)38 ± 10.4Right 30, 60, 120, 240, 2000Sch < Contr at LI 60Sch < Contr
Ludewig and Vollenweider[30]Medicated Sch (19)/Contr (24). Tested 3 times with 1-month intervalSch 42 ± 9; Contr 30 ± 10Right 30, 60, 120, 240, 2000Sch < Contr at LI 60Sch < Contr, only in the first block (out of three in total)
Düring et al[31]Unmedicated Sch (52)/Contr (47)25.4 ± 4.2Right 60, 120Sch < Contr at LI 60 and 120NA
Csomor et al[32]Unmedicated Sch (14)/Contr (46)26.1 ± 1.0Right 60, 120Sch < Contr at LI 60 NA
Aggernaes et al[33]Unmedicated Sch (31)/Contr (31)25.6 ± 0.8Right 60, 120Sch < Contr at LI 60NA
Hammer et al[34]Unmedicated Sch (13)/Contr (17)29.3 ± 5.4Right 30, 60, 120Sch < Contr at LI 60NA
Bo et al[35]Sch (31)/High risk of psychosis/Contr (31)23.9 ± 2.1Right 60, 120Sch < Contr at LI 60NA
Hedberg et al[36]Unmedicated Sch (49)/Contr (35)27.8 ± 0.9Right 30, 60, 120Sch < Contr at LI 60NA
Wang et al[37]Unmedicated Sch (35)/Contr (35)20.4 ± 4.5Right 30, 60, 120Sch < Contr at LI 60NA
Storozheva et al[38]Unmedicated Sch (26 men)/Contr (25 men)25-50Right and left60, 120, 2500Sch < Contr at LI 60 (right and left)Sch < Contr, only in the first of two PPI blocks, at the left eye
Storozheva et al[39]Unmedicated Sch (35 men)/Contr (26 men)35.3 ± 10.5; 26.4 ± 5.7Right and left60, 120, 2500Sch < Contr at LI 60, 120 (right and left)No difference
Kirenskaya et al[40]Unmedicated violent Sch (44)/non violent Sch (27)/Contr (48)30.2 ± 6.93; 29.4 ± 7.0; 27.6 ± 3.6Right and left60, 120, 2500Violent Sch < contr at LI 60 ms, bilateral; Non violent Sch< at LI 60 ms, right eyeIn violent Sch; No difference from Contr; Non violent Sch < Contr; Non violent < violent Sch at both eyes, in 1st PPI block
Cadenhead et al[41]Medicated Sch (23)/Sch relatives (34)/shizotypal (11)/Contr (25)34.4 ± 8.5; 46.1 ± 16.1; 44.4 ± 9.0; 41.1 ± 16.7Right and left30, 120Sch < Contr; Sch at LI 30 ms, right eye relatives < Contr at LI 30 ms bilaterally; Shizotypal < Contr at LI 30 ms bilaterallyNA
Wynn et al[43]Medicated Sch (90)/Sch siblings (48)/Contr (47)42.70 ± 8.46; 38.23 ± 9.61; 35.64 ± 7.96Left120, 4500No differenceSch < Contr; Siblings < Contr
Table 2 Summary of studies on prepulse inhibition and prepulse facilitation of the startle response in the cohorts of participants with mood disorders, mean ± SD.
Ref.
Groups (group size)
Mean age
Side of recording
Lead interval, ms
PPI
PPF
Matsuo et al[63]Non-manic BD (63 depressed + 43 euthymic)/ Contr (232)39.4 ± 8.3Left 60, 120BD depressed; Men < Contr at LI 120 ms NA
Perry et al[64]Psychotic BD (15)/Sch (16), Contr (17)34.4 ± 9.7Right30, 60, 120Sch < Contr; BD < Contr at LI 60 ms and 120 msNA
Perry et al[65]MDD (15)/Contr (17)34.1 ± 8.3Right30, 60, 120No differenceNA
Quednow et al[66]MDD (20), Contr (18)35.4 ± 8.3Right150No differenceNA
Barrett et al[67]Euthymic BD (23)/Contr (20)44.4 ± 10.2Left60, 120No differenceNA
Rich et al[68]Children with BD (16)/Contr (17)13.1 ± 2.3Right60, 120No differenceNA
Carroll et al[69]BD (32)/Contr (35)34.5 ± 7.9Right60, 120No differenceNA
Giakoumaki et al[70]BD (21) BD/siblings (21)/Contr (19)18-50 (range)Right60, 120BD < Contr; BD siblings < Contr at LI 60 ms and 120 msNA
Gogos et al[71]BD (31)/Contr (35)21-61 (range)Right60, 120Men BD < Contr at LI 60 ms; Women BD > Contr at LI 120 msNA
Ivleva et al[72]BD (38)/Sch (62) relatives (32 Sch and 29 BD)/Contr (53)40.58 ± 10.73; 36.18 ± 10.37; 41.97 ± 10.84; 32.19 ± 14.98; 36.84 ± 11.35Right120, 4500No differenceNo difference
Sánchez-Morla et al[73]BD (52)/Contr (50)70.9 ± 8.3Right60, 120BD < Contr at LI 60 ms and 120 msNA
Matsuo et al[74]MDD (221)/Contr (250)18-64 (range)Left60, 120Men MDD < Contr at LI 120 ms; Women no differenceNA
Massa et al[75]Sch (143), Sch-Fam (178), SAD (123), SAD-Fam (152), BD (138), BD-Fam(183), Contr (226)37.87 ± 12.77; 35.49 ± 13.48; 40.17 ± 15.89; 34.13 ± 11.93; 43.47 ± 15.87; 38.28 ± 12.17; 39.43 ± 16.28Right120, 4500No differenceNo difference
Table 3 Summary of studies on prepulse inhibition and prepulse facilitation of the startle response in cohorts of participants with trauma- and anxiety-related disorders, mean ± SD.
Ref.
Groups (group size)
Mean age
Side of recording
Lead interval, ms
PPI
PPF
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Grillon et al[76]Unmedicated PTSD veterans (21)/non PTSD veterans) (11)/non-PTSD civilians (17)41.3 ± 5.0Left 120PTSD < non-PTSD civiliansNA
Grillon et al[77]Unmedicated PTSD veterans (34)/non PTSD veterans) (17)/non-PTSD civilians (14)44.1 ± 3.8Left 120PTSD < non-PTSD civiliansNA
Grillon et al[78]Unmedicated PTSD veterans (21)/non PTSD veterans (15)/non-PTSD civilians (22)47.6 ± 3.6Left 120PTSD < non-PTSD civiliansNA
Butler et al[79]No data on medication PTSD veterans (20)/non PTSD veterans (18)40.9 ± 4.4Right 120No differenceNA
Ornitz et al[80]Unmedicated PTSD children (6)/control children (6)8-13 (range)Left 120, 250, 800, 2000 PTSD < control (at LI 120)PTSD > control
Lipschitz et al[81]PTSD girls (28)/control girls (23)16.5 ± 2.2Left 120, 2000 No differenceNo difference
Holstein et al[82]PTSD patients (27)/control (24)38.6 ± 2.8Right 60, 120, 2000No differenceNo difference
Pineles et al[83]PTSD women (22)/trauma control women (25)31.98 (9.7)Left 120PTSD < controlNA
Meteran et al[84]Refugees with PTSD (25)/refugees without PTSD (20)45.8 ± 7.3Right 60, 120No differenceNA
Acheson et al[85]Combatants with PTSD (46)/combatants without PTSD (1182)22.7 ± 1.6Left 30, 60, 120PTSD < combatants without PTSD, at LI 30 ms and 60 msNA
Echiverri-Cohen et al[86]PTSD (28)/trauma without PTSD (27)/control (19)32.3 ± 5.8Left 30, 60, 120PTSD < trauma without PTSD and control at LI 30 ms and 60 msNA
Panic disorders
Ludewig et al[87]Panic disorder (20)/control (21)31.5 ± 10.0Right 30, 60, 120, 240 2000Panic disorder < control at LI 30, 60, 120, 240 msNo difference
Ludewig et al[88]Panic disorder unmedicated (14)/panic disorder medicated (24)/control (28)35 ± 8.1Right 30, 60, 120, 240 2000Panic disorder < control at LI 30, 60, 120, 240 msNo difference
Larsen et al[89]Panic disorder (12)/social phobia (22)/control (15)33.6 ± 9.2Right 100No differenceNA
Storozheva et al[50]General anxiety disorder + panic disorder (69)/control (50)31.8 ± 0.9Left and right60, 120 2500 No differencePatients < control
Anxiety traits
Duley et al[49]High anxiety trait (15)/low (23) anxiety trait21.2 ± 0.24Left and right 30, 60, 120High < low at 30, 60, 120 ms averaged from both eyesNA
De Pascalis et al[90]High anxiety trait (15)/low (23) anxiety trait24.6 ± 3.8Left 30, 60, 120No difference (higher amplitude in high anxiety group)NA
McMillan et al[91]High (25)/low (25) anxiety sensitivity trait22.8 ± 3.1Left 120High < lowNA
Nocturnal enuresis
Ornitz et al[92]ADHD children (30)/NE children (13)/NE + ADHD children (17)/control children (42)6-11 (range)Left 60, 120, 4000 (control)NE < non NE + ADHD, NE + ADHD < control (at LI 120 ms)No difference
Table 4 Summary of studies on prepulse inhibition and prepulse facilitation in the cohorts of participants with obsessive-compulsive disorder, mean ± SD.
Ref.
Groups (group size)
Mean age
Side of recording
Lead interval, ms
PPI
PPF
Hoenig et al[93]OCD (34)/Contr (33)31.5 ± 1.6Right120OCD < ContrNA
Swerdlow et al[94]OCD (34)/Contr (33)36.2 ± 2.1Right100OCD < ContrNA
de Leeuw et al[95]Unmedicated OCD (25)/Contr (25)32 ± 7.4Right100No differenceNA
Ahmari et al[96]Unmedicated OCD (22)/Contr (22)31 ± 6.3Right100OCD < ContrNA
Schleyken et al[97]Treatment refractory OCD (15)/Contr (21)20-64 (range)Right60, 120, 200No differenceNA
Steinman et al[98]Unmedicated OCD (45)/Contr (62)28.2 ± 4.3Right120OCD < Contr (in female only)NA
Table 5 Summary of studies on prepulse inhibition and prepulse facilitation of the startle response in the cohorts of participants with Tourette syndrome, mean ± SD.
Ref.
Groups (group size)
Mean age
Side of recording
Lead interval, ms
PPI
PPF
Castellanos et al[52]ADHD (7)/ADHD + TS (7)/Contr (14)11.4 ± 1.7Left and right30, 60, 120, 250 (tactile pulse and prepulse)ADHD + TS < Contr ADHD + TS < ADHD across all LINA
Swerdlow et al[53]TS (10)/Contr (14)9-17 (range)Left and right120 (tactile pulse and prepulse)TS < ContrNA
Schleyken et al[97]Treatment refractory TS (6)/Contr (21)20-64 (range)Right60, 120, 200 (80 dB)No differenceNA
Buse et al[99]TS (22)/Contr (22)13.5 ± 1.62Left140 (tactile pulse and prepulse)TS < ContrNA
Zebardast et al[100]TS (17)/Contr (16)30.4 ± 8.9Left100 (tactile pulse and prepulse)No differenceNA
Table 6 Summary of studies on prepulse inhibition and prepulse facilitation of the startle response in cohorts of participants with autistic spectrum disorders, mean ± SD.
Ref.
Groups (group size)
Mean age
Side of recording
Lead interval, ms
PPI
PPF
McAlonan et al[101]Autistic (12)/Contr (14)32.5 ± 8.2Right30, 60, 120Autistic < Contr at LI 120 msNA
Perry et al[102]Autistic (14)/Contr (16)28.7 ± 5.4Right30, 60, 120Autistic < Contr at LI 60 msNA
Yuhas et al[103]FXS - A (17)/FXS + A (15)/IA (15)/Contr (18)16.4 ± 1.8Not specified60, 120, 240FXS - A < Contr FXS + A < Contr IA vs Contr; no diffferenceNA
Sinclair et al[104]MCDD (11)/autistic (12)/Contr (11)11.4 ± 1.3; 11.8 ± 1.7; 11.1 ± 1.9Not specified120No differenceNA
Kohl et al[105]Autistic (17)/Contr (17)41.8 ± 8.1Right60, 120, 240No differenceNA
Madsen et al[106]Autistic (18)/Contr (34)11.2 ± 0.8Right60, 120Autistic > Contr at LI 120 msNA
Takahashi et al[107]Autistic (17)/Contr (27)11.3 ± 1.2Right120No difference in PPI, ASR amplitude; autistic > Contr at low pulse intensityNA
Table 7 Summary of studies on prepulse inhibition and prepulse facilitation of the startle response in cohorts of participants with neurodegenerative disorders, mean ± SD.
Ref.
Groups (group size)
Mean age
Side of recording
Lead interval, ms
PPI
PPF
Aygün et al[108]REM sleep disorders (19)/Contr (17)43.8 ± 7.60Left200REM sleep disorders < ContrNA
Zoetmulder et al[109]REM sleep disorders (12)/PD (40)/MSA (10)/Contr (20) 56.3 ± 13.0; 62.7 ± 6.6; 63.4 ± 6.7; 56.4 ± 10.0Right30, 60, 120, 300MSA < Contr at 60 ms and 120 ms NA
Hanzlíková et al[110]Functiomal movement disorders (22)/Contr (22)47.5 ± 9.8Left and right100 (electrical stimulation)Functional movement disorders < ContrNA
Millian-Morell et al[111]PD (54)/Contr (35)70.4 ± 9.8Right60, 120, 1000PD > Contr at LI 120 msPD > Contr
Aziz[112]Mild AD (20)/first degree (20)/Contr (30)79.4 ± 4.7Right30, 60, 120, 2000Mild AD < Contr/first degree < Contr (trend) at LI 120 msNo difference
Ueki et al[113]Mild AD (20)/first degree relatives (20)/Contr (30)70.5 ± 5.7Right50Mild AD < Contr/first degree relatives > ContrNA
Salem et al[114]AD + MCI (22)/Contr (33)72.6 ± 4.3Right30, 60, 120 No differenceNA
Hejl et al[115]AD (26)/MCI (22)/Contr (49)73.1 ± 3.5Right30, 60, 120 No differenceNA
Swerdlow et al[116]HD (22)/Contr (22)45.2 ± 6.4Right30, 60, 120 acoustic and tactileHD < Contr across al LINA
Schizophrenia

The most studied nosology is schizophrenia, and the results of some investigations in this cohort are shown in Table 1. Data on the effect of therapy on prepulse changes in SR are conflicting[24,25,33,61], so we tried to provide data from patients who were never medicated or who had discontinued medication. The exceptions were the studies that assessed PPF and those that assessed SR bilaterally.

The results of 18 studies are shown in Table 1. The protocols of all studies presented included assessment of the PPI at LI 120 ms, and a PPI deficit was observed in patients with schizophrenia in 5 studies (28%)[25-27,31,39]. Of the 15 studies in which the PPI was tested at LI 60 ms[25,27-40,43]. 14 (93%) showed a deficit in patients with schizophrenia. In one study[25], the PPI decrease in patients did not reach the level of statistical significance, although according to the figures it could be considered at the trend level. The PPI at LI 30 ms was tested in 9 studies[25,27-30,34,36,37,41] and its decrease in patients was found in 3 studies (33%). These results suggest that the most pronounced PPI deficit in schizophrenia is observed at LI 60 ms, which is consistent with the results of the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia multicentre study[42] (but see also the meta-analysis by San-Martin et al)[62].

The fact that the vast majority of researchers have recorded EMG from the orbicularis oculi of the right eye complicates the analysis of another PPI parameter, the laterality of the observed deficit.

In the studies conducted by our team using bilateral registration in patients weaned off medication, a reduction in the PPI was observed in both eyes[38-40]. However, it should be noted that these studies included patients who had committed crimes. A crime-specific analysis showed that a reduction in the PPI in the left eye was only observed in patients with a history of aggressive crime (homicide or aggravated assault), whereas right-sided PPI deficits showed no association with the characteristics of the crime[40]. Cadenhead et al[41] also used bilateral recording and analyzed data from the right and left eyes separately at 30 and 120 ms LI. A decrease in the PPI was found in the group of patients at LI 30 ms, which was more pronounced in the right than in the left eye. In a study where only the left eye was recorded, no PPI differences were found between controls and patients; however, it should be noted that the length of the LI was 120 ms[63].

Of the 7 studies that assessed PPF[28-30,38-40,43], 6 found a deficit in facilitation in schizophrenic patients. The decrease in the PPF was mostly observed in the first blocks of the test session, when the conditioned reflex for that time had not yet developed. There was no clear lateralization of the PPF deficit.

Mood disorders

The results of the studies in patients with mood disorders are shown in Table 2. It includes data from patients with BD, major depressive disorder, schizoaffective disorder (SAD), and relatives of patients with BD or SAD[63-75].

All of these studies included treated patients, as no data are available in the literature on studies of untreated or withdrawn patients with mood disorders. Three studies[64,72,75] that also included patients with schizophrenia were of particular interest, as they tested across nosologies under similar experimental conditions. Unfortunately, two of these studies found no differences between experimental groups and controls in either PPI at LI 120 ms or PPF at LI 4500 ms[72,75]. In contrast, Perry et al[64] found reduced PPI at LI 60 ms and 120 ms in both schizophrenia and BD patients.

Overall, 6 out of 13 studies (46%) found a PPI deficit in patients. In all of these studies, a decrease in the PPI was found at LI 120[63,64,70,71,73,74] and in 4 of these studies it was also found at LI 60 ms[64,70,71,73]. Thus, in contrast to schizophrenia, it cannot be concluded that there is a predominant deficit in pre-attentive gating in patients with mood disorders.

With regard to lateralization, it should be noted that in the studies with depressed patients, the PPI deficit was only observed in males at LI 120 ms and in those experiments where the side of registration was left but not right[74].

Only 2 studies assessed the PPF[72,75], showing no differences between controls and mood disorder patients.

In general, PPI deficits in mood disorders are less marked than in schizophrenia and show a significant dependence on the phase of the illness as well as on the gender of the subjects, so that females with BD may even show an increase in the PPI compared to controls.

Trauma- and anxiety-related disorders

Among trauma and anxiety disorders, PTSD is the most studied[76-86]. Notably, the majority of studies on PTSD (8 out of 11) recorded EMG from the left eye (Table 3), and differences in the PPI between patients and controls were mainly (7 out of 8 studies) observed in conditions where the left eye was assessed[76-78,80,83,84,86].

Due to the large variability in the LI sets used in the studies with PTSD cohorts, it is difficult to determine the dependence of the observed PPI changes in PTSD on the length of the LI. It should also be noted that most studies of PTSD in military personnel have found significant differences at a LI length of 120 ms when compared to civilians without PTSD, but not when compared to military personnel without PTSD[76-78]. A study by Acheson et al[85] using LI 30, 60, and 120 ms found a reduced PPI in military personnel with PTSD compared to military personnel without PTSD, and this deficit was observed at LI 30 and 60, but not 120 ms[85].

Very similar results were found by Echiverri-Cohen et al[86], who recruited an additional control group of individuals with trauma experience but without PTSD, and used LIs of 30, 60, and 120 ms. As in the Acheson et al’s study[85], PPI deficits were observed in individuals with PTSD at LIs of 30 and 60, but not 120 ms[86]. The observed phenomenon of a left-sided decrease in the PPI at short intervals is of interest for further studies of the mechanisms of PTSD and the diagnostic validity of the test.

There is a paucity of data on changes in PPF in PTSD; it is cautious to assume that it is not deficient in this disorder. There is also a paucity of research on the prepulse modification of ASR in other types of anxiety disorders[87-92]. Their results suggest impairments in sensorimotor gating and attentional allocation, which may be observed when registering from the left eye, but they do not allow conclusions to be drawn about the effects of interval length.

Obsessive-compulsive disorders

The results of the studies of prepulse modification of ASR in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder[93-98] do not allow us to assess the effects of laterality of registration and length of LI (Table 4). In all the studies, registration was performed from the right eye and (with one exception) with LI lengths of 100 or 120 ms. In 3 of the 6 studies[93,94,96], a PPI decrease was found in patients of both sexes, and in one, the most representative study by Steinman et al[98], the deficit was found only in females. Taken together, the results of these studies suggest that patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder have a reduced PPI when recorded from the right eye and in the 100-120 ms time interval (early attentional stage).

Tourette syndrome

Deficits in Tourette syndrome were observed only in children and adolescents[52,53,97,99,100], and not in adult participants (Table 5). It should be noted that tactile stimulation was used in these studies. The SR was recorded from the left eye or bilaterally with response averaging. From the data obtained, it can be concluded that in children and adolescents with TS, a PPI decrease can be observed predominantly from the left eye with LI in the range of 100-140 ms.

Autistic spectrum disorders

Only 2[101,102] of the 7 studies[101-107] found PPI deficits in individuals with autism (Table 6). One study found reduced PPI in patients with fragile X syndrome, but not in individuals with autism[103]. Remarkably, two studies found increased PPI in children with autism[106,107]. Thus, we cannot be certain about PPI decrease in autistic spectrum disorders, and the question of the pattern of its changes in this nosology needs further study.

Neurodegenerative disorders

We analyzed data on changes in the PPI in the most common neurodegenerative disorders[108-116], such as Alzheimer’s disease and synucleinopathies (Parkinson’s disease, Lewy body dementia, Multiple System Atrophy), as well as in preclinical conditions which may represent a prodrome for the development of neurodegeneration, such as rapid eye movement (REM) sleep disorders and functional movement disorders (Table 7).

Studies of PPI in patients with preclinical signs of possible synucleinopathies have shown a PPI decrease at LI of 60 ms and in the range 100-200 ms[108-110]. Paradoxically, the available, albeit limited, data suggest that such a decrease is more pronounced in younger patients, whereas in the group of Parkinson’s patients aged 70 years, there is no decrease but an increase in the PPI and PPF. The authors of this study proposed that the excessive modulation of ASR by prepulse in Parkinson’s disease may reflect an impaired integration of sensory inputs, with a lack of coordination between preparatory and executive motor commands[111]. In patients with early Alzheimer’s disease, impaired PPI was observed in 2[112,113] out of 4[112-115] studies, and no patterns of LI length and laterality could be identified. Surprisingly, relatives of Alzheimer’s disease patients showed a PPI increase with a 50 ms LI[113].

DATA OBTAINED UNDER CONDITIONS OF ACTIVATED ATTENTION

Some studies have shown that the effects of selective and non-selective attention on the prepulse modification of SR depend on the length of the LI and may manifest differently in healthy individuals and in psychopathology[3,55-57,59,60,117].

However, the results of these studies are highly dependent on the details of the protocols used, particularly how attention is induced. Until recently, studies using directed attention paradigms have primarily been conducted with healthy participants or patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders[54-57].

An exception was a study on children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. They showed deficits in prepulse modulation in controlled attention conditions[58]. More recently, some researchers have used the PPI under perceived spatial separation (PSS-PPI) paradigm, which relies on the effect of spatial separation of the prepulse and background noise sources to increase attention to the prepulse, compared to the PPI inder perceived spatial co-location (PSC-PPI) paradigm, in which the background noise and prepulse sources are not separated and attention to the prepulse is not increased[35,118-120].

It was found that in the PSC-PPI test, a statistically significant (P = 0.01) PPI deficit was observed only in individuals with first-episode schizophrenia and only at an LI of 60 ms, whereas clinical high-risk participants did not differ from controls. In the PSS-PPI test, a highly significant (P < 0.001) PPI deficit was observed in both patient groups at both 60 ms and 120 ms LIs. In the PSC-PPI test, a statistically significant (P = 0.01) PPI deficit was observed only in first-episode schizophrenia patients and only at 60 ms LIs, whereas clinical high-risk participants did not differ from controls. In the PSS-PPI test, a highly significant (P < 0.001) PPI deficit was observed in both patient groups at both 60 ms and 120 ms LIs[35]. Thus, testing with simultaneous application of PSS-PPI and PSC-PPI allowed us to identify and differentiate PPI deficits in both groups of patients.

Differences were also found only in the PSS-PPI mode, but not in the PSC-PPI mode, when comparing patients with BD and healthy controls for the PPI level at LI 120 ms[118].

However, in another study by the same authors involving three groups of patients: First-episode schizophrenia, BD and major depressive disorder, significant PPI differences from the control level on the PSS-PPI test after Bonferroni correction were observed only in patients with schizophrenia[120]. It can be suggested that the combination of PSS-PPI and PSC-PPI modes may be a promising approach in differential diagnosis.

PREPULSE MODULATION OF NEURAL COMPONENTS OF STARTLE

Some researchers consider the registration of event-related auditory potentials, particularly the N1/P2 complex, as one of the ways to increase the sensitivity and specificity of the assessment of ASR prepulse changes. This approach has been made possible by advances in encephalogram analysis techniques, particularly the reduction of signal contamination[121]. The use of this method significantly increases the sensitivity of PPI estimation, especially when combined with PSS-PPI and PSC-PPI stimulation regimes[119].

In a study on groups of patients with schizophrenia and BD, using a ‘standard’ stimulation protocol and simultaneous EMG and EEG analysis, it was shown that the PPI deficit estimated by EMG parameters was observed only in the schizophrenia group at LI 60 and 120 ms, whereas when assessing the P2-N1 index of auditory evoked potentials, a PPI decrease relative to control was observed in bipolar and schizophrenia patients at LI 30 and 60 ms[122]. Thus, this direction seems promising for finding specific changes in the prestimulus modification of ASR in different disorders and needs to be further developed.

INTEGRATED ASSESSMENT OF PHYSIOLOGICAL PARAMETERS AND GENETIC POLYMORPHISM

Patterns of association between PPI scores and the activity of different brain structures show some degree of specificity in healthy populations and in different psychopathologies[17]. Accordingly, the neurotransmitter mechanisms that determine the specifics of sensorimotor information processing and attention may differ in different psychopathologies. In turn, the activity of neurotransmission in different brain structures depends significantly on the activity of genes encoding receptors and enzymes of neurotransmitter metabolism.

Genetic studies have shown a high degree of heritability of PPI[41], and a meta-analysis of the association between genetic polymorphisms and PPI, performed in a pooled sample of healthy subjects and patients with schizophrenia, found some polymorphic variants with significant effects[123]. One of these polymorphisms, namely rs4680, Val/158Met substitution in the gene catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), the main enzyme of dopamine catabolism in the prefrontal cortex, has been investigated in several other studies, also showing its association with PPI scores. Studies in healthy populations showed that the highest PPI levels were observed in methionine homozygotes and the lowest in the Val/Val genotype, while the Val/Met variant showed intermediate levels[123-127]. Polynomial contrast analysis showed a linear relationship between PPI levels and Val allele load[124].

The data on the effect of the rs4680 COMT polymorphism on PPI, obtained in a cohort of patients with schizophrenia, are significantly different from those obtained in healthy individuals.

In a Chinese sample of patients with a first episode of schizophrenia, no effect of rs4680 on PPI was found at LI 120 ms[128]. A study of Russian patients with schizophrenia also found no effect of rs4680 on PPI at LI 60 and 120 ms[125]. In a Caucasian cohort of patients with schizophrenia, Quednow et al[129] found an effect of rs4680 on PPI. In contrast to healthy individuals, genotype-dependent PPI analysis in schizophrenia patients showed a quadratic rather than linear trend, such that higher PPI levels were observed in Met/Met genotype carriers, whereas the difference between Val/Met and Val/Val and between Met/Met and Val/Val was not significant. These results suggest that reduced PPI in Met/Met genotype carriers can be considered a schizophrenia-specific defect, whereas the comparison of PPI levels in Val/Val genotype carriers does not allow us to do so with certainty. Further research in this direction may prove fruitful for the development of a diagnostic tool.

A similar pattern is observed when analyzing the effects of polymorphisms of the gene encoding the alpha-3 subunit of nicotinic receptors, namely polymorphisms rs1051730-cytosine (C) replacement by thymine (T) and rs1317286-adenine (A) replacement by guanine (G). In healthy individuals, the association of rs1051730 and rs1317286 with PPIs was described by a linear trend (CC > CT > TT and AA > GA > GG respectively). At the same time, a significant quadratic trend was observed in patients with schizophrenia with respect to the maximum values of PPIs in CT/GA heterozygotes[130]. These results also suggest that it is important to take genotype into account when comparing PPIs in healthy and schizophrenic patients.

The T102C polymorphism of the serotonin receptor 5-HT2A gene was also found to have opposite associations with PPI levels in healthy and schizophrenic subjects. Patients with the TT genotype have the highest PPI levels, whereas in healthy individuals the presence of the T allele is associated with reduced PPI at LI 50 and, to a lesser extent, at LI 150 ms[131]. Although similar studies have not been performed for other disorders, it could be proposed that polymorphic gene variants that specifically contribute to the regulation of prepulse modification of ASR are also found in these disorders.

CONCLUSION

The phenomenon of prepulse modification of the SR provides an opportunity to study the mechanisms of sensorimotor information processing and attention, as well as their impairment in various neurological and psychiatric disorders. Paradoxically, such a relatively simple behavioral phenomenon has multiple complex regulatory mechanisms. This is related to the importance of SR for the realization of optimal individual behavior in a threatening situation with a high degree of uncertainty. At the same time, the brain structures involved in the regulation of PPI and PPF show different changes in activity in different pathologies, which may lead to changes in the modification of the prepulse that are specific to certain disorders. The detection of such specificity requires the application of uniform schemes, including bilateral recording with separate scoring of data on both sides, the use of comparable sets of LIs, including those that condition SR facilitation, within widely used ‘standard’ methods of stimulation and recording of the SR. It should be noted that elevated PPI levels may be observed in some pathologies. In particular, this has been observed in medication-naive subjects with early psychosis and those at risk of developing psychosis[132], in females with BD[71], in autistic adolescents (Madsen), in elderly patients with Parkinson’s disease[111], and in first-degree relatives of patients with Alzheimer’s disease[113]. Some researchers consider such increases in the PPIs to be evidence of compensatory processes[132]. However, Millian-Morell et al[111], who has observed both PPI and PPF elevations in elderly PD patients, sees these coupled phenomena as evidence of the disintegration of the various stages of sensorimotor information processing. Thus, the inclusion of the long-lead PPF mode appears to be extremely useful in the development of an optimal protocol for ‘standard’ testing of the prepulse modification of the SR. EEG assessment, including evoked potentials and changes in oscillation patterns, appears to be a promising new approach to estimating PPI and PPF. The application of new modes of stimulation, including the paradigm of pattern separation, could also be very useful. Finally, an integrated assessment of physiological parameters and genetic polymorphisms is proposed as very useful both for diagnosis and for delineating the mechanisms of neuropsychiatric disorders. The evidence to date suggests that the use of the SR and its prepulse modification in a battery of neurophysiological screening tools can increase the efficiency of identifying people in need of psychological and psychiatric care, assessing the dynamics of the state and the impact of therapy. The achievement of an acceptable level of sensitivity and specificity of such neurophysiological test systems can be ensured by a combination of different paradigms providing, in addition to the assessment of sensorimotor gating and integration, the assessment of sensory filtering (suppression of the evoked potential P50), as well as various indicators of voluntary attention (evoked potentials P300 and N400) and executive control (antisaccade test, continuous performance test, etc.). Furthermore, the analysis of data arrays accumulated over many years in studies of different cohorts of subjects, as well as the results of recent studies using new experimental approaches, indicate the possibility of increasing the sensitivity and specificity of the method of prepulse modification of the SR for the diagnosis, prognosis and testing the effectiveness of treatment of mental and neurological disorders.

Footnotes

Provenance and peer review: Invited article; Externally peer reviewed.

Peer-review model: Single blind

Specialty type: Psychiatry

Country of origin: Russia

Peer-review report’s classification

Scientific Quality: Grade A, Grade A

Novelty: Grade B, Grade B

Creativity or Innovation: Grade B, Grade B

Scientific Significance: Grade B, Grade C

P-Reviewer: Stoyanov D S-Editor: Fan M L-Editor: A P-Editor: Wang CH

References
1.  Graham FK. Presidential Address, 1974. The more or less startling effects of weak prestimulation. Psychophysiology. 1975;12:238-248.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 856]  [Cited by in RCA: 862]  [Article Influence: 17.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
2.  Braff DL, Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR. Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2001;156:234-258.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1350]  [Cited by in RCA: 1329]  [Article Influence: 55.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
3.  Jennings PD, Schell AM, Filion DL, Dawson ME. Tracking early and late stages of information processing: contributions of startle eyeblink reflex modification. Psychophysiology. 1996;33:148-155.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 39]  [Cited by in RCA: 43]  [Article Influence: 1.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
4.  Davis M  The Mammalian Startle Response. In: Eaton RC. Neural Mechanisms of Startle Behavior. Boston: Springer, 1984: 287-351.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
5.  Koch M. The neurobiology of startle. Prog Neurobiol. 1999;59:107-128.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 945]  [Cited by in RCA: 977]  [Article Influence: 37.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
6.  Blumenthal TD, Cuthbert BN, Filion DL, Hackley S, Lipp OV, van Boxtel A. Committee report: Guidelines for human startle eyeblink electromyographic studies. Psychophysiology. 2005;42:1-15.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 793]  [Cited by in RCA: 838]  [Article Influence: 41.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
7.  Gehricke JG, Ornitz EM, Siddarth P. Differentiating between reflex and spontaneous blinks using simultaneous recording of the orbicularis oculi electromyogram and the electro-oculogram in startle research. Int J Psychophysiol. 2002;44:261-268.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 14]  [Cited by in RCA: 14]  [Article Influence: 0.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
8.  Haerich P. Using airpuffs to elicit the human blink reflex. Behav Res Methods. 1998;30:661-666.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 9]  [Cited by in RCA: 8]  [Article Influence: 0.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
9.  Cranney J, Cohen ME. The glabella startle reflex: inhibition by frequency and intensity modulations. Percept Psychophys. 1985;37:28-34.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 3]  [Cited by in RCA: 3]  [Article Influence: 0.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
10.  Braff DL, Grillon C, Geyer MA. Gating and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49:206-215.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 759]  [Cited by in RCA: 773]  [Article Influence: 23.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
11.  Swerdlow NR, Eastvold A, Gerbranda T, Uyan KM, Hartman P, Doan Q, Auerbach P. Effects of caffeine on sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex in normal control subjects: impact of caffeine intake and withdrawal. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2000;151:368-378.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 40]  [Cited by in RCA: 42]  [Article Influence: 1.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
12.  Norris CM, Blumenthal TD. A relationship between inhibition of the acoustic startle response and the protection of prepulse processing. Psychobiology. 1996;24:160-168.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]
13.  Blumenthal TD, Reynolds JZ, Spence TE. Support for the interruption and protection hypotheses of prepulse inhibition of startle: evidence from a modified Attention Network Test. Psychophysiology. 2015;52:397-406.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 20]  [Cited by in RCA: 23]  [Article Influence: 2.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
14.  Rohleder C, Jung F, Mertgens H, Wiedermann D, Sué M, Neumaier B, Graf R, Leweke FM, Endepols H. Neural correlates of sensorimotor gating: a metabolic positron emission tomography study in awake rats. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:178.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 21]  [Cited by in RCA: 22]  [Article Influence: 2.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
15.  Rohleder C, Wiedermann D, Neumaier B, Drzezga A, Timmermann L, Graf R, Leweke FM, Endepols H. The Functional Networks of Prepulse Inhibition: Neuronal Connectivity Analysis Based on FDG-PET in Awake and Unrestrained Rats. Front Behav Neurosci. 2016;10:148.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 39]  [Cited by in RCA: 49]  [Article Influence: 5.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
16.  Jafari Z, Kolb BE, Mohajerani MH. Prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex and P50 gating in aging and alzheimer's disease. Ageing Res Rev. 2020;59:101028.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 14]  [Cited by in RCA: 28]  [Article Influence: 5.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
17.  Naysmith LF, Kumari V, Williams SCR. Neural mapping of prepulse-induced startle reflex modulation as indices of sensory information processing in healthy and clinical populations: A systematic review. Hum Brain Mapp. 2021;42:5495-5518.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 9]  [Cited by in RCA: 12]  [Article Influence: 3.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
18.  Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Geyer MA. Sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex: what we said 25 years ago, what has happened since then, and what comes next. J Psychopharmacol. 2016;30:1072-1081.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 148]  [Cited by in RCA: 141]  [Article Influence: 15.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
19.  Poje AB, Filion DL. Effects of prepulse format and lead interval on the assessment of automatic and attention-modulated prepulse inhibition. Cogn Process. 2021;22:559-567.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1]  [Cited by in RCA: 4]  [Article Influence: 1.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
20.  Dawson ME, Schell AM, Swerdlow NR, Filion DL.   Cognitive, clinical, and neurophysiological implications of startle modification. In: Lang PJ, Simons RF, Balaban MT. Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes. New Jersey: Routledge, 1997: 257-279.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]
21.  Putnam LE, Vanman EJ.   Long Lead Interval Startle Modification. In: Dawson ME, Schell AM, Bohmelt AH, editors. Startle Modification: Implications for Neuroscience, Cognitive Science, and Clinical Science. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1999: 72-92.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]
22.  Filion DL, Dawson ME, Schell AM. Modification of the acoustic startle-reflex eyeblink: a tool for investigating early and late attentional processes. Biol Psychol. 1993;35:185-200.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 162]  [Cited by in RCA: 165]  [Article Influence: 5.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
23.  Neuner I, Stöcker T, Kellermann T, Ermer V, Wegener HP, Eickhoff SB, Schneider F, Shah NJ. Electrophysiology meets fMRI: neural correlates of the startle reflex assessed by simultaneous EMG-fMRI data acquisition. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31:1675-1685.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 9]  [Cited by in RCA: 22]  [Article Influence: 1.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
24.  Molina V, López DE, Villa R, Pérez J, Martín C, Ballesteros A, Cardoso A, Sancho C. Prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex in schizophrenia remains stable with short-term quetiapine. Eur Psychiatry. 2011;26:271-275.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 3]  [Cited by in RCA: 5]  [Article Influence: 0.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
25.  Xue YY, Wang HN, Xue F, Tan QR. Atypical antipsychotics do not reverse prepulse inhibition deficits in acutely psychotic schizophrenia. J Int Med Res. 2012;40:1467-1475.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 10]  [Cited by in RCA: 13]  [Article Influence: 1.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
26.  Hammer TB, Oranje B, Skimminge A, Aggernæs B, Ebdrup BH, Glenthøj B, Baaré W. Structural brain correlates of sensorimotor gating in antipsychotic-naive men with first-episode schizophrenia. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2013;38:34-42.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 24]  [Cited by in RCA: 28]  [Article Influence: 2.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
27.  Mackeprang T, Kristiansen KT, Glenthoj BY. Effects of antipsychotics on prepulse inhibition of the startle response in drug-naïve schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52:863-873.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 117]  [Cited by in RCA: 121]  [Article Influence: 5.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
28.  Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Vollenweider FX. Deficits in prepulse inhibition and habituation in never-medicated, first-episode schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:121-128.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 199]  [Cited by in RCA: 200]  [Article Influence: 9.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
29.  Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Etzensberger M, Vollenweider FX. Stability of the acoustic startle reflex, prepulse inhibition, and habituation in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2002;55:129-137.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 89]  [Cited by in RCA: 90]  [Article Influence: 3.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
30.  Ludewig K, Vollenweider FX. Impaired sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia with deficit and with nondeficit syndrome. Swiss Med Wkly. 2002;132:159-165.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in RCA: 1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
31.  Düring S, Glenthøj BY, Andersen GS, Oranje B. Effects of dopamine D2/D3 blockade on human sensory and sensorimotor gating in initially antipsychotic-naive, first-episode schizophrenia patients. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:3000-3008.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 38]  [Cited by in RCA: 42]  [Article Influence: 3.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
32.  Csomor PA, Yee BK, Feldon J, Theodoridou A, Studerus E, Vollenweider FX. Impaired prepulse inhibition and prepulse-elicited reactivity but intact reflex circuit excitability in unmedicated schizophrenia patients: a comparison with healthy subjects and medicated schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:244-255.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 32]  [Cited by in RCA: 37]  [Article Influence: 2.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
33.  Aggernaes B, Glenthoj BY, Ebdrup BH, Rasmussen H, Lublin H, Oranje B. Sensorimotor gating and habituation in antipsychotic-naive, first-episode schizophrenia patients before and after 6 months' treatment with quetiapine. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;13:1383-1395.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 43]  [Cited by in RCA: 50]  [Article Influence: 3.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
34.  Hammer TB, Oranje B, Fagerlund B, Bro H, Glenthøj BY. Stability of prepulse inhibition and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenia: a 6-year follow-up study of initially antipsychotic-naive, first-episode schizophrenia patients. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;14:913-925.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 26]  [Cited by in RCA: 29]  [Article Influence: 2.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
35.  Bo Q, Mao Z, Tian Q, Yang N, Li X, Dong F, Zhou F, Li L, Wang C. Impaired Sensorimotor Gating Using the Acoustic Prepulse Inhibition Paradigm in Individuals at a Clinical High Risk for Psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2021;47:128-137.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 6]  [Cited by in RCA: 11]  [Article Influence: 2.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
36.  Hedberg M, Imbeault S; Karolinska Schizophrenia Project (KaSP) consortium, Erhardt S, Schwieler L. Disrupted sensorimotor gating in first-episode psychosis patients is not affected by short-term antipsychotic treatment. Schizophr Res. 2021;228:118-123.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 5]  [Cited by in RCA: 6]  [Article Influence: 1.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
37.  Wang ZR, Tan YL, Yang FD, Zhang WF, Zou YZ, Tan SP, Song CS, Li YL, Zhang WH, Zhou DF. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in Chinese patients with first-episode, medication-naïve schizophrenia. Chin Med J (Engl). 2013;126:526-531.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
38.  Storozheva ZI, Kirenskaya AV, Lazarev IE, Novototskii-vlasov VY, Samylkin DV, Fastovtsov GA. Prepulse Modification of the Acoustic Startle Reaction in Healthy Subjects and Patients with Schizophrenia. Neurosci Behav Physi. 2012;42:128-132.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]
39.  Storozheva ZI, Kirenskaya AV, Novototsky-Vlasov VY, Telesheva KY, Pletnikov M. Startle Modification and P50 Gating in Schizophrenia Patients and Controls: Russian Population. Span J Psychol. 2016;19:E8.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 15]  [Cited by in RCA: 16]  [Article Influence: 1.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
40.  Kirenskaya AV, Storozheva ZI, Ilyushina EA, Tkatchenko AA. [The comparative study of the early stages of sensory and sensory-motor information processing in aggressive and non-aggressive schizophrenic patients]. Zh Nevrol Psikhiatr Im S S Korsakova. 2020;120:45-52.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in RCA: 1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
41.  Cadenhead KS, Swerdlow NR, Shafer KM, Diaz M, Braff DL. Modulation of the startle response and startle laterality in relatives of schizophrenic patients and in subjects with schizotypal personality disorder: evidence of inhibitory deficits. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1660-1668.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 270]  [Cited by in RCA: 261]  [Article Influence: 10.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
42.  Swerdlow NR, Light GA, Thomas ML, Sprock J, Calkins ME, Green MF, Greenwood TA, Gur RE, Gur RC, Lazzeroni LC, Nuechterlein KH, Radant AD, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Sugar CA, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI, Braff DL. Deficient prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia in a multi-site cohort: Internal replication and extension. Schizophr Res. 2018;198:6-15.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 36]  [Cited by in RCA: 47]  [Article Influence: 6.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
43.  Wynn JK, Dawson ME, Schell AM, McGee M, Salveson D, Green MF. Prepulse facilitation and prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia patients and their unaffected siblings. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:518-523.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 68]  [Cited by in RCA: 71]  [Article Influence: 3.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
44.  Santos-Carrasco D, De la Casa LG. Prepulse inhibition deficit as a transdiagnostic process in neuropsychiatric disorders: a systematic review. BMC Psychol. 2023;11:226.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in RCA: 11]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
45.  Swerdlow NR, Weber M, Qu Y, Light GA, Braff DL. Realistic expectations of prepulse inhibition in translational models for schizophrenia research. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2008;199:331-388.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 453]  [Cited by in RCA: 434]  [Article Influence: 25.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
46.  Schall U, Schön A, Zerbin D, Bender S, Eggers C, Oades RD. A left temporal lobe impairment of auditory information processing in schizophrenia: an event-related potential study. Neurosci Lett. 1997;229:25-28.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 18]  [Cited by in RCA: 19]  [Article Influence: 0.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
47.  Kumari V, Antonova E, Geyer MA. Prepulse inhibition and "psychosis-proneness" in healthy individuals: an fMRI study. Eur Psychiatry. 2008;23:274-280.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 54]  [Cited by in RCA: 61]  [Article Influence: 12.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
48.  Campbell LE, Hughes M, Budd TW, Cooper G, Fulham WR, Karayanidis F, Hanlon MC, Stojanov W, Johnston P, Case V, Schall U. Primary and secondary neural networks of auditory prepulse inhibition: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of sensorimotor gating of the human acoustic startle response. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;26:2327-2333.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 63]  [Cited by in RCA: 62]  [Article Influence: 3.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
49.  Duley AR, Hillman CH, Coombes S, Janelle CM. Sensorimotor gating and anxiety: prepulse inhibition following acute exercise. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;64:157-164.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 17]  [Cited by in RCA: 24]  [Article Influence: 1.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
50.  Storozheva ZI, Akhapkin RV, Bolotina OV, Korendrukhina A, Novototsky-Vlasov VY, Shcherbakova IV, Kirenskaya AV. Sensorimotor and sensory gating in depression, anxiety, and their comorbidity. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2021;22:183-193.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 4]  [Cited by in RCA: 9]  [Article Influence: 2.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
51.  Hasenkamp W, Epstein MP, Green A, Wilcox L, Boshoven W, Lewison B, Duncan E. Heritability of acoustic startle magnitude, prepulse inhibition, and startle latency in schizophrenia and control families. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178:236-243.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 61]  [Cited by in RCA: 61]  [Article Influence: 4.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
52.  Castellanos FX, Fine EJ, Kaysen D, Marsh WL, Rapoport JL, Hallett M. Sensorimotor gating in boys with Tourette's syndrome and ADHD: preliminary results. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;39:33-41.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 296]  [Cited by in RCA: 294]  [Article Influence: 10.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
53.  Swerdlow NR, Karban B, Ploum Y, Sharp R, Geyer MA, Eastvold A. Tactile prepuff inhibition of startle in children with Tourette's syndrome: in search of an "fMRI-friendly" startle paradigm. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:578-585.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 138]  [Cited by in RCA: 143]  [Article Influence: 6.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
54.  Hazlett EA, Romero MJ, Haznedar MM, New AS, Goldstein KE, Newmark RE, Siever LJ, Buchsbaum MS. Deficient attentional modulation of startle eyeblink is associated with symptom severity in the schizophrenia spectrum. Schizophr Res. 2007;93:288-295.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 52]  [Cited by in RCA: 46]  [Article Influence: 2.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
55.  Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS, Zhang J, Newmark RE, Glanton CF, Zelmanova Y, Haznedar MM, Chu KW, Nenadic I, Kemether EM, Tang CY, New AS, Siever LJ. Frontal-striatal-thalamic mediodorsal nucleus dysfunction in schizophrenia-spectrum patients during sensorimotor gating. Neuroimage. 2008;42:1164-1177.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 78]  [Cited by in RCA: 67]  [Article Influence: 3.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
56.  Hazlett EA, Levine J, Buchsbaum MS, Silverman JM, New A, Sevin EM, Maldari LA, Siever LJ. Deficient attentional modulation of the startle response in patients with schizotypal personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1621-1626.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 27]  [Cited by in RCA: 27]  [Article Influence: 1.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
57.  Kedzior KK, Martin-Iverson MT. Attention-dependent reduction in prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex in cannabis users and schizophrenia patients--a pilot study. Eur J Pharmacol. 2007;560:176-182.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 30]  [Cited by in RCA: 32]  [Article Influence: 1.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
58.  Conzelmann A, Pauli P, Mucha RF, Jacob CP, Gerdes AB, Romanos J, Bähne CG, Heine M, Boreatti-Hümmer A, Alpers GW, Fallgatter AJ, Warnke A, Lesch KP, Weyers P. Early attentional deficits in an attention-to-prepulse paradigm in ADHD adults. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010;119:594-603.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 25]  [Cited by in RCA: 28]  [Article Influence: 1.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
59.  Filion DL, Poje AB. Selective and nonselective attention effects on prepulse inhibition of startle: a comparison of task and no-task protocols. Biol Psychol. 2003;64:283-296.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 42]  [Cited by in RCA: 47]  [Article Influence: 2.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
60.  Scholes KE, Martin-Iverson MT. Disturbed prepulse inhibition in patients with schizophrenia is consequential to dysfunction of selective attention. Psychophysiology. 2010;47:223-235.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 26]  [Cited by in RCA: 32]  [Article Influence: 2.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
61.  Kumari V, Soni W, Mathew VM, Sharma T. Prepulse inhibition of the startle response in men with schizophrenia: effects of age of onset of illness, symptoms, and medication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:609-614.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 158]  [Cited by in RCA: 170]  [Article Influence: 6.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
62.  San-Martin R, Castro LA, Menezes PR, Fraga FJ, Simões PW, Salum C. Meta-Analysis of Sensorimotor Gating Deficits in Patients With Schizophrenia Evaluated by Prepulse Inhibition Test. Schizophr Bull. 2020;46:1482-1497.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 44]  [Cited by in RCA: 46]  [Article Influence: 9.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
63.  Matsuo J, Ota M, Hidese S, Teraishi T, Hori H, Ishida I, Hiraishi M, Kunugi H. Sensorimotor Gating in Depressed and Euthymic Patients with Bipolar Disorder: Analysis on Prepulse Inhibition of Acoustic Startle Response Stratified by Gender and State. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:123.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 11]  [Cited by in RCA: 13]  [Article Influence: 1.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
64.  Perry W, Minassian A, Feifel D, Braff DL. Sensorimotor gating deficits in bipolar disorder patients with acute psychotic mania. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:418-424.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 204]  [Cited by in RCA: 205]  [Article Influence: 8.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
65.  Perry W, Minassian A, Feifel D. Prepulse inhibition in patients with non-psychotic major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2004;81:179-184.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 43]  [Cited by in RCA: 41]  [Article Influence: 2.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
66.  Quednow BB, Westheide J, Kühn KU, Werner P, Maier W, Hawellek B, Wagner M. Normal prepulse inhibition and habituation of acoustic startle response in suicidal depressive patients without psychotic symptoms. J Affect Disord. 2006;92:299-303.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 30]  [Cited by in RCA: 31]  [Article Influence: 1.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
67.  Barrett SL, Kelly C, Watson DR, Bell R, King DJ. Normal levels of prepulse inhibition in the euthymic phase of bipolar disorder. Psychol Med. 2005;35:1737-1746.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 29]  [Cited by in RCA: 31]  [Article Influence: 1.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
68.  Rich BA, Vinton D, Grillon C, Bhangoo RK, Leibenluft E. An investigation of prepulse inhibition in pediatric bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2005;7:198-203.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 27]  [Cited by in RCA: 29]  [Article Influence: 1.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
69.  Carroll CA, Vohs JL, O'donnell BF, Shekhar A, Hetrick WP. Sensorimotor gating in manic and mixed episode bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9:221-229.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 27]  [Cited by in RCA: 26]  [Article Influence: 1.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
70.  Giakoumaki SG, Roussos P, Rogdaki M, Karli C, Bitsios P, Frangou S. Evidence of disrupted prepulse inhibition in unaffected siblings of bipolar disorder patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:1418-1422.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 51]  [Cited by in RCA: 57]  [Article Influence: 3.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
71.  Gogos A, van den Buuse M, Rossell S. Gender differences in prepulse inhibition (PPI) in bipolar disorder: men have reduced PPI, women have increased PPI. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2009;12:1249-1259.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 41]  [Cited by in RCA: 47]  [Article Influence: 2.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
72.  Ivleva EI, Moates AF, Hamm JP, Bernstein IH, O'Neill HB, Cole D, Clementz BA, Thaker GK, Tamminga CA. Smooth pursuit eye movement, prepulse inhibition, and auditory paired stimuli processing endophenotypes across the schizophrenia-bipolar disorder psychosis dimension. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40:642-652.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 35]  [Cited by in RCA: 41]  [Article Influence: 3.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
73.  Sánchez-Morla EM, Mateo J, Aparicio A, García-Jiménez MÁ, Jiménez E, Santos JL. Prepulse inhibition in euthymic bipolar disorder patients in comparison with control subjects. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2016;134:350-359.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 12]  [Cited by in RCA: 19]  [Article Influence: 2.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
74.  Matsuo J, Ota M, Hidese S, Hori H, Teraishi T, Ishida I, Hiraishi M, Kunugi H. Sexually dimorphic deficits of prepulse inhibition in patients with major depressive disorder and their relationship to symptoms: A large single ethnicity study. J Affect Disord. 2017;211:75-82.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 8]  [Cited by in RCA: 9]  [Article Influence: 1.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
75.  Massa N, Owens AV, Harmon W, Bhattacharya A, Ivleva EI, Keedy S, Sweeney JA, Pearlson GD, Keshavan MS, Tamminga CA, Clementz BA, Duncan E. Relationship of prolonged acoustic startle latency to diagnosis and biotype in the bipolar-schizophrenia network on intermediate phenotypes (B-SNIP) cohort. Schizophr Res. 2020;216:357-366.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 7]  [Cited by in RCA: 12]  [Article Influence: 2.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
76.  Grillon C, Morgan CA, Southwick SM, Davis M, Charney DS. Baseline startle amplitude and prepulse inhibition in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 1996;64:169-178.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 152]  [Cited by in RCA: 163]  [Article Influence: 5.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
77.  Grillon C, Morgan CA 3rd, Davis M, Southwick SM. Effects of experimental context and explicit threat cues on acoustic startle in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44:1027-1036.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 189]  [Cited by in RCA: 193]  [Article Influence: 7.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
78.  Grillon C, Morgan CA 3rd, Davis M, Southwick SM. Effect of darkness on acoustic startle in Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:812-817.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in RCA: 37]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
79.  Butler RW, Braff DL, Rausch JL, Jenkins MA, Sprock J, Geyer MA. Physiological evidence of exaggerated startle response in a subgroup of Vietnam veterans with combat-related PTSD. Am J Psychiatry. 1990;147:1308-1312.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 171]  [Cited by in RCA: 173]  [Article Influence: 4.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
80.  Ornitz EM, Pynoos RS. Startle modulation in children with posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1989;146:866-870.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 158]  [Cited by in RCA: 125]  [Article Influence: 3.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
81.  Lipschitz DS, Mayes LM, Rasmusson AM, Anyan W, Billingslea E, Gueorguieva R, Southwick SM. Baseline and modulated acoustic startle responses in adolescent girls with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:807-814.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 36]  [Cited by in RCA: 38]  [Article Influence: 1.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
82.  Holstein DH, Vollenweider FX, Jäncke L, Schopper C, Csomor PA. P50 suppression, prepulse inhibition, and startle reactivity in the same patient cohort suffering from posttraumatic stress disorder. J Affect Disord. 2010;126:188-197.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 46]  [Cited by in RCA: 57]  [Article Influence: 3.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
83.  Pineles SL, Blumenthal TD, Curreri AJ, Nillni YI, Putnam KM, Resick PA, Rasmusson AM, Orr SP. Prepulse inhibition deficits in women with PTSD. Psychophysiology. 2016;53:1377-1385.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 22]  [Cited by in RCA: 39]  [Article Influence: 4.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
84.  Meteran H, Vindbjerg E, Uldall SW, Glenthøj B, Carlsson J, Oranje B. Startle habituation, sensory, and sensorimotor gating in trauma-affected refugees with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med. 2019;49:581-589.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 13]  [Cited by in RCA: 17]  [Article Influence: 2.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
85.  Acheson DT, Baker DG, Nievergelt CM, Yurgil KA, Geyer MA, Risbrough VB. Prospective longitudinal assessment of sensorimotor gating as a risk/resiliency factor for posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47:2238-2244.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 4]  [Cited by in RCA: 10]  [Article Influence: 3.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
86.  Echiverri-Cohen AM, Zoellner LA, Ho W, Husain J. An analysis of inhibitory functioning in individuals with chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2016;37:94-103.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 8]  [Cited by in RCA: 13]  [Article Influence: 1.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
87.  Ludewig S, Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Hell D, Vollenweider FX. Prepulse inhibition deficits in patients with panic disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2002;15:55-60.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 120]  [Cited by in RCA: 125]  [Article Influence: 5.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
88.  Ludewig S, Geyer MA, Ramseier M, Vollenweider FX, Rechsteiner E, Cattapan-Ludewig K. Information-processing deficits and cognitive dysfunction in panic disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2005;30:37-43.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
89.  Larsen DK, Norton GR, Walker JR, Stein MB. Analysis of Startle Responses in Patients with Panic Disorder and Social Phobia. Cogn Behav Ther. 2002;31:156-169.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 28]  [Cited by in RCA: 29]  [Article Influence: 1.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
90.  De Pascalis V, Cozzuto G, Russo E. Effects of personality trait emotionality on acoustic startle response and prepulse inhibition including N100 and P200 event-related potential. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;124:292-305.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 25]  [Cited by in RCA: 27]  [Article Influence: 2.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
91.  McMillan KA, Asmundson GJ, Zvolensky MJ, Carleton RN. Startle response and anxiety sensitivity: subcortical indices of physiologic arousal and fear responding. Emotion. 2012;12:1264-1272.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 13]  [Cited by in RCA: 13]  [Article Influence: 1.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
92.  Ornitz EM, Hanna GL, de Traversay J. Prestimulation-induced startle modulation in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and nocturnal enuresis. Psychophysiology. 1992;29:437-451.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 98]  [Cited by in RCA: 95]  [Article Influence: 2.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
93.  Hoenig K, Hochrein A, Quednow BB, Maier W, Wagner M. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1153-1158.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 143]  [Cited by in RCA: 146]  [Article Influence: 7.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
94.  Swerdlow NR, Benbow CH, Zisook S, Geyer MA, Braff DL. A preliminary assessment of sensorimotor gating in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1993;33:298-301.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 267]  [Cited by in RCA: 250]  [Article Influence: 7.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
95.  de Leeuw AS, Oranje B, van Megen HJ, Kemner C, Westenberg HG. Sensory gating and sensorimotor gating in medication-free obsessive-compulsive disorder patients. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2010;25:232-240.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 17]  [Cited by in RCA: 24]  [Article Influence: 1.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
96.  Ahmari SE, Risbrough VB, Geyer MA, Simpson HB. Impaired sensorimotor gating in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1216-1223.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 137]  [Cited by in RCA: 148]  [Article Influence: 11.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
97.  Schleyken S, Baldermann J, Huys D, Franklin J, Visser-Vandewalle V, Kuhn J, Kohl S. Deep brain stimulation and sensorimotor gating in tourette syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2020;129:272-280.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 6]  [Cited by in RCA: 12]  [Article Influence: 2.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
98.  Steinman SA, Ahmari SE, Choo T, Kimeldorf MB, Feit R, Loh S, Risbrough V, Geyer MA, Steinglass JE, Wall M, Schneier FR, Fyer AJ, Simpson HB. PREPULSE INHIBITION DEFICITS ONLY IN FEMALES WITH OBSESSIVE-COMPULSIVE DISORDER. Depress Anxiety. 2016;33:238-246.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 12]  [Cited by in RCA: 19]  [Article Influence: 2.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
99.  Buse J, Beste C, Herrmann E, Roessner V. Neural correlates of altered sensorimotor gating in boys with Tourette Syndrome: A combined EMG/fMRI study. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2016;17:187-197.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 48]  [Cited by in RCA: 53]  [Article Influence: 5.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
100.  Zebardast N, Crowley MJ, Bloch MH, Mayes LC, Wyk BV, Leckman JF, Pelphrey KA, Swain JE. Brain mechanisms for prepulse inhibition in adults with Tourette syndrome: initial findings. Psychiatry Res. 2013;214:33-41.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 43]  [Cited by in RCA: 49]  [Article Influence: 4.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
101.  McAlonan GM, Daly E, Kumari V, Critchley HD, van Amelsvoort T, Suckling J, Simmons A, Sigmundsson T, Greenwood K, Russell A, Schmitz N, Happe F, Howlin P, Murphy DG. Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gating in Asperger's syndrome. Brain. 2002;125:1594-1606.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 335]  [Cited by in RCA: 314]  [Article Influence: 13.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
102.  Perry W, Minassian A, Lopez B, Maron L, Lincoln A. Sensorimotor gating deficits in adults with autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:482-486.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 262]  [Cited by in RCA: 259]  [Article Influence: 14.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
103.  Yuhas J, Cordeiro L, Tassone F, Ballinger E, Schneider A, Long JM, Ornitz EM, Hessl D. Brief report: Sensorimotor gating in idiopathic autism and autism associated with fragile X syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:248-253.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 56]  [Cited by in RCA: 56]  [Article Influence: 4.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
104.  Sinclair D, Oranje B, Razak KA, Siegel SJ, Schmid S. Sensory processing in autism spectrum disorders and Fragile X syndrome-From the clinic to animal models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;76:235-253.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 115]  [Cited by in RCA: 138]  [Article Influence: 17.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
105.  Kohl S, Wolters C, Gruendler TO, Vogeley K, Klosterkötter J, Kuhn J. Prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex in high functioning autism. PLoS One. 2014;9:e92372.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 43]  [Cited by in RCA: 48]  [Article Influence: 4.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
106.  Madsen GF, Bilenberg N, Cantio C, Oranje B. Increased prepulse inhibition and sensitization of the startle reflex in autistic children. Autism Res. 2014;7:94-103.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 54]  [Cited by in RCA: 62]  [Article Influence: 5.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
107.  Takahashi H, Komatsu S, Nakahachi T, Ogino K, Kamio Y. Relationship of the Acoustic Startle Response and Its Modulation to Emotional and Behavioral Problems in Typical Development Children and Those with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46:534-543.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 28]  [Cited by in RCA: 32]  [Article Influence: 3.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
108.  Aygün D, Ertaş Fİ, Gündüz A, Benbir Şenel G, Karadeniz D, Kızıltan M. The role of pedunculopontine nucleus in isolated REM sleep behavior disorder and REM sleep without atonia. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2021;84:68-73.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1]  [Cited by in RCA: 3]  [Article Influence: 0.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
109.  Zoetmulder M, Biernat HB, Nikolic M, Korbo L, Jennum PJ. Sensorimotor gating deficits in multiple system atrophy: comparison with Parkinson's disease and idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014;20:297-302.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 12]  [Cited by in RCA: 19]  [Article Influence: 1.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
110.  Hanzlíková Z, Kofler M, Slovák M, Věchetová G, Fečíková A, Kemlink D, Sieger T, Růžička E, Valls-Solé J, Edwards MJ, Serranová T. Prepulse inhibition of the blink reflex is abnormal in functional movement disorders. Mov Disord. 2019;34:1022-1030.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 3]  [Cited by in RCA: 13]  [Article Influence: 2.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
111.  Millian-Morell L, Lopez-Alburquerque T, Rodriguez-Rodriguez A, Gomez-Nieto R, Carro J, Meilan JJG, Martinez-Sanchez F, Sancho C, Lopez DE. Relations between Sensorimotor Integration and Speech Disorders in Parkinson's Disease. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2018;15:149-156.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 6]  [Cited by in RCA: 9]  [Article Influence: 1.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
112.  Aziz VM. Deficit in Sensorimotor Gating in Alzheimer’s Disease (AD): Measuring Pre Pulse Inhibition (PPI) as a Measure of Liability to AD. Ann Psychiatr Clin Neurosci. 2019;2:1008.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
113.  Ueki A, Goto K, Sato N, Iso H, Morita Y. Prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle response in mild cognitive impairment and mild dementia of Alzheimer type. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;60:55-62.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 53]  [Cited by in RCA: 50]  [Article Influence: 2.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
114.  Salem LC, Hejl AM, Garde E, Leffers AM, Paulson OB, Waldemar G. White matter hyperintensities and prepulse inhibition in a mixed elderly population. Psychiatry Res. 2011;194:314-318.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 6]  [Cited by in RCA: 7]  [Article Influence: 0.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
115.  Hejl AM, Glenthøj B, Mackeprang T, Hemmingsen R, Waldemar G. Prepulse inhibition in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:1045-1050.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 45]  [Cited by in RCA: 42]  [Article Influence: 2.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
116.  Swerdlow NR, Paulsen J, Braff DL, Butters N, Geyer MA, Swenson MR. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic and tactile startle response in patients with Huntington's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1995;58:192-200.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 327]  [Cited by in RCA: 334]  [Article Influence: 11.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
117.  Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS, Tang CY, Fleischman MB, Wei TC, Byne W, Haznedar MM. Thalamic activation during an attention-to-prepulse startle modification paradigm: a functional MRI study. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:281-291.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 70]  [Cited by in RCA: 69]  [Article Influence: 2.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
118.  Bo Q, Mao Z, Tian Q, Wen Y, Dong F, Li X, Wang Z, Ma X, Wang C. Deficits of perceived spatial separation-induced prepulse inhibition in patients with bipolar disorder compared to healthy controls. J Affect Disord. 2018;240:63-71.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 6]  [Cited by in RCA: 7]  [Article Influence: 1.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
119.  Lei M, Zhang C, Li L. Neural correlates of perceptual separation-induced enhancement of prepulse inhibition of startle in humans. Sci Rep. 2018;8:472.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 13]  [Cited by in RCA: 17]  [Article Influence: 2.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
120.  Sun Y, Bo Q, Mao Z, Tian Q, Dong F, Li L, Wang C. Different levels of prepulse inhibition among patients with first-episode schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2024;49:E1-E10.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in RCA: 2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
121.  San-Martin R, Zimiani MI, Noya C, Ávila MAV, Shuhama R, Del-Ben CM, Menezes PR, Fraga FJ, Salum C. A Method for Simultaneous Evaluation of Muscular and Neural Prepulse Inhibition. Front Neurosci. 2018;12:654.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 6]  [Cited by in RCA: 11]  [Article Influence: 1.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
122.  San-Martin R, Zimiani MI, de Ávila MAV, Shuhama R, Del-Ben CM, Menezes PR, Fraga FJ, Salum C. Early Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder Patients Display Reduced Neural Prepulse Inhibition. Brain Sci. 2022;12:93.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 4]  [Cited by in RCA: 8]  [Article Influence: 2.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
123.  Quednow BB, Ejebe K, Wagner M, Giakoumaki SG, Bitsios P, Kumari V, Roussos P. Meta-analysis on the association between genetic polymorphisms and prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response. Schizophr Res. 2018;198:52-59.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 25]  [Cited by in RCA: 27]  [Article Influence: 3.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
124.  Roussos P, Giakoumaki SG, Rogdaki M, Pavlakis S, Frangou S, Bitsios P. Prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex depends on the catechol O-methyltransferase Val158Met gene polymorphism. Psychol Med. 2008;38:1651-1658.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 62]  [Cited by in RCA: 66]  [Article Influence: 3.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
125.  Kirenskaya AV, Storozheva ZI, Kolobov VV, Sherstnev VV. The acoustic startle response and polymorphism of the gene for catechol-O-methyltransferase in the norm and in schizophrenia. Neurochem J. 2015;9:76-83.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 5]  [Cited by in RCA: 6]  [Article Influence: 0.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
126.  Quednow BB, Schmechtig A, Ettinger U, Petrovsky N, Collier DA, Vollenweider FX, Wagner M, Kumari V. Sensorimotor gating depends on polymorphisms of the serotonin-2A receptor and catechol-O-methyltransferase, but not on neuregulin-1 Arg38Gln genotype: a replication study. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:614-620.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 63]  [Cited by in RCA: 64]  [Article Influence: 4.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
127.  Wu C, Ding Y, Chen B, Gao Y, Wang Q, Wu Z, Lu L, Luo L, Zhang C, Bao X, Yang P, Fan L, Lei M, Li L. Both Val158Met Polymorphism of Catechol-O-Methyltransferase Gene and Menstrual Cycle Affect Prepulse Inhibition but Not Attentional Modulation of Prepulse Inhibition in Younger-Adult Females. Neuroscience. 2019;404:396-406.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 11]  [Cited by in RCA: 16]  [Article Influence: 2.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
128.  Liu X, Hong X, Chan RC, Kong F, Peng Z, Wan X, Wang C, Cheng L. Association study of polymorphisms in the alpha 7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit and catechol-o-methyl transferase genes with sensory gating in first-episode schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2013;209:431-438.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 15]  [Cited by in RCA: 17]  [Article Influence: 1.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
129.  Quednow BB, Wagner M, Mössner R, Maier W, Kühn KU. Sensorimotor gating of schizophrenia patients depends on Catechol O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36:341-346.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 46]  [Cited by in RCA: 48]  [Article Influence: 3.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
130.  Petrovsky N, Quednow BB, Ettinger U, Schmechtig A, Mössner R, Collier DA, Kühn KU, Maier W, Wagner M, Kumari V. Sensorimotor gating is associated with CHRNA3 polymorphisms in schizophrenia and healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1429-1439.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 67]  [Cited by in RCA: 67]  [Article Influence: 4.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
131.  Bräuer D, Strobel A, Hensch T, Diers K, Lesch KP, Brocke B. Genetic variation of serotonin receptor function affects prepulse inhibition of the startle. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2009;116:607-613.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 18]  [Cited by in RCA: 18]  [Article Influence: 1.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
132.  Cadenhead KS. Startle reactivity and prepulse inhibition in prodromal and early psychosis: effects of age, antipsychotics, tobacco and cannabis in a vulnerable population. Psychiatry Res. 2011;188:208-216.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 43]  [Cited by in RCA: 43]  [Article Influence: 3.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]