Minireviews Open Access
Copyright ©The Author(s) 2025. Published by Baishideng Publishing Group Inc. All rights reserved.
World J Psychiatry. May 19, 2025; 15(5): 103967
Published online May 19, 2025. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v15.i5.103967
Catatonia and autism spectrum disorder: A common comorbid syndrome or a core feature?
Yassir Mahgoub, Dallas Hamlin, Andrew Francis, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Health, Penn State College of Medicine, Hershey, PA 17033, United States
Hailey Kindt, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA 19107, United States
ORCID number: Yassir Mahgoub (0000-0003-3934-3484).
Author contributions: Mahgoub Y contributed to conceptualization and writing the original draft; Hamlin D contributed to conceptualization, writing, reviewing, and editing; Kindt H contributed to reviewing and editing; Francis A contributed to supervision, reviewing, and editing.
Conflict-of-interest statement: The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Open Access: This article is an open-access article that was selected by an in-house editor and fully peer-reviewed by external reviewers. It is distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited, and the use is non-commercial. See: https://creativecommons.org/Licenses/by-nc/4.0/
Corresponding author: Yassir Mahgoub, MD, Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Health, Penn State College of Medicine, 500 University Dr, Hershey, PA 17033, United States. ymahgoub@pennstatehealth.psu.edu
Received: December 6, 2024
Revised: February 27, 2025
Accepted: March 28, 2025
Published online: May 19, 2025
Processing time: 146 Days and 6.1 Hours

Abstract

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is an early-onset neurodevelopmental disorder marked by persistent deficits in social communication and interaction, alongside restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviors (RRB), interests, or activities. It often co-occurs with various neuropsychiatric disorders, though their frequency varies widely due to unclear boundaries between the core features of ASD and common comorbidities. Catatonia, increasingly noted in neurodevelopmental conditions like ASD, shares striking similarities with ASD in symptomatology, brain mechanisms, and treatment responses, prompting the question of whether it is a core feature of ASD or a distinct condition. This paper delved into this overlap, exploring the relationship between catatonia and ASD through a narrative review of peer-reviewed literature from 1943 to 2024, sourced from PubMed and psychiatric journals. Focusing on ASD diagnostic evolution, symptom overlap with catatonia, and shared neurobiological and therapeutic characteristics, we used thematic analysis to synthesize findings into key areas such as historical nosology, phenomenological overlap, neurobiological parallels, and treatment response. The evidence revealed weak support for separating catatonia from overlapping RRB features of ASD, suggesting that some RRB might align more with comorbid catatonia than intrinsic ASD traits. However, this idea needs further validation through rigorous clinical trials. Clarifying this relationship could refine diagnostic approaches and open doors to targeted treatments, potentially improving outcomes for those affected.

Key Words: Catatonia; Autism spectrum disorder; Psychopathology; Electroconvulsive therapy; Stereotypies; Mannerisms

Core Tip: Catatonia and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) share similarities in phenomenology, neurobiology, and treatment response, with catatonia occurring frequently with ASD. Evidence linking these shared features to the core definition of ASD remains weak. Viewing catatonia as a distinct comorbid condition offers a more convincing framework. ASD and catatonia can be differentiated by detecting changes in severity or patterns to identify catatonia. Yet this leaves chronic, unchanging cases unresolved. Resolving this distinction is crucial for clinical care and research, as it could reveal new treatment options, like lorazepam or electroconvulsive therapy, for restrictive and repetitive behaviors in ASD, which were previously dismissed as intrinsic and untreatable due to their persistence.



INTRODUCTION

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is an early-onset neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by persistent deficits in social communication and social interaction in addition to restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviors (RRB), interests, or activities[1]. The nosology of ASD has undergone several changes over the last 100 years, following the broader changes in general psychiatric nosology and the recognition of wide variation in disease severity and comorbidities.

ASD is commonly comorbid with several neuropsychiatric disorders, and the prevalence of these comorbidities varies widely, partly due to difficulty delineating between core features of ASD and comorbidities. Mannion and Leader[2] found that 46.1% of children and adolescents with ASD have comorbid conditions, but when intellectual disability was added, this prevalence rose to 78.7%. These comorbidities include attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in 50%-90%[3,4], major depressive disorder or bipolar disorder in 30%[5], anxiety and obsessive-compulsive disorder in 2%-40%[6], epilepsy in 30%[7], gastrointestinal symptoms in 9%-91%, sleep problems in up to 80%[8], and intellectual disability among 30% of ASD patients[9].

Concurrently, catatonia has prominent affective, cognitive, motor, and behavioral manifestations[10]. Catatonia is common among patients with structural central nervous system abnormalities and developmental disorders, with the combined rate of catatonia at 11.6%[11]. ASD is among the neuropsychiatric disorders with a high frequency of catatonia, with recent studies estimating the frequency to be 4%-17%[12].

Catatonia shares significant phenomenological and clinical overlap with the domain of RRB, interests, or activities in ASD. This can be elucidated in presentations like manneristic speech, manneristic and stereotypical behaviors, echolalia, echopraxia, perseveration, verbigeration, posturing, rituals, and compulsions. Other features, such as excitement and agitation, are seen in both disorders despite not being a core feature of ASD. In addition to the clinical features, ASD and catatonia share overlapping neurobiological features and similar responses to the treatment modalities, such as lorazepam and electroconvulsive therapy (ECT)[13-15].

The extensive similarities between catatonia and ASD in their nosological development, increased prevalence of catatonia in ASD populations, overlap in clinical presentation, neurobiology, and response to treatment modalities spark a pivotal question: Are these repetitive behaviors an intrinsic part of ASD, or do they signal comorbid catatonia? To unravel this, we conducted a narrative review of literature from 1943 to 2024, sourced from PubMed and psychiatric journals. We synthesized the literature on ASD and catatonia using Braun and Clarke’s six-step thematic analysis framework[16]. We identified recurring concepts through iterative reading and generated initial codes [e.g., 'echolalia', 'γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) dysfunction']. These were grouped into provisional themes aligned with our aims: Tracing historical nosology; analyzing phenomenological overlap; and evaluating their distinction as separate entities. These themes were refined by cross-checking the literature, resulting in four final themes: Historical nosology; phenomenological overlap; neurobiological parallels; and treatment response. These were integrated into a narrative synthesis, supported by examples such as the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-5-TR criteria and SHANK3 mutation studies, to explore the ASD-catatonia relationship.

Early insights suggest the line between ASD’s RRB and catatonia blurs. RRB often resemble catatonic features, hinting they might lean more toward a separate condition than a core trait. Resolving this could reshape diagnosis and unlock targeted therapies, though their safety in ASD remains a cautious frontier needing deeper study.

HISTORICAL BACKGROUND OF ASD NOSOLOGY
Early concepts

Both ASD and catatonia were assimilated into schizophrenia until DSM-III when ASD became a separate condition within developmental disorders[17]. Catatonia, on the other hand, was a subtype or a presentation related to schizophrenia until DSM-5 when it was finally recognized as a specifier to several psychiatric and medical conditions[18].

The development of ASD nosology followed an interesting path. Early descriptions of ASD started with Bleuler[19] in 1911, who described symptoms in severe cases of schizophrenia. Based on predominant theories of that era on putative psychodynamic mechanisms, Bleuler provided the term “autism” to describe a hypothesized state of infantile wishes to avoid unsatisfying realities and replace them with fantasies and hallucinations[20]. This term was imprecise in distinguishing between what would become known as ASD and early-onset schizophrenia.

By 1943, Kanner[21,22] was the first to separate autism from childhood schizophrenia following the careful observation of 11 children who had a typical pattern of extreme aloneness from the beginning of life and an anxiously obsessive desire for the perseveration of sameness. Kanner[21,22] agreed with some aspects of Bleuler’s initial description of the withdrawal from reality but hypothesized that patients with autism engaged in withdrawal willingly and surrendered themselves to a fantasy. He also believed that children with autism never developed the capacity to interact appropriately with the external world, distinguishing them from children who suffered regression due to another cause. In addition, Kanner[21,22] hypothesized that individuals with ASD developed a unique and relatively unskilled relationship with the intimate external environment, thus separating it from the withdrawal of the external world described by Bleuler[19]. Kanner[21,22] excluded children with known genetic syndromes, brain injuries, or severe intellectual disability when diagnosing children with autism.

In 1949, Mahler[23] described a syndrome of “symbiotic infantile psychosis” in children she believed made desperate efforts to avoid the catastrophic anxiety of separation, focusing on attachment theory, which was different from Kanner’s conceptualization of autism. In 1964, Rimland[24] attempted to refine autism diagnostic criteria by creating a checklist that had 109 items that covered several aspects of developmental history and milestones, peculiarities about interactions with the external world, and several symptoms that are currently recognized under the RRB domains, such as rituals, verbigeration, and echolalia.

Leonhard[25] studied children with psychomotor psychosis and catatonia. He suggested that catatonia during childhood only occurs among infantile schizophrenia, a rare variant of schizophrenia that appears during childhood. This was thought to progress to systematized schizophrenia, a form equivalent to some cases of adult-onset schizophrenia. He also recognized that infantile autism, which Kanner described, was a competing concept to his infantile schizophrenia hypothesis. Hence, he suggested that children with schizophrenia are seen “to have no soul, and nothing can be inferred from their facial expressions, while those with infantile autism have a different soul”[26]. DSM-III accepted both diagnoses, but due to the considerable overlap in criteria, it led to poor recognition of autism disorder during that period[27].

DSM evolution and the shadow of schizophrenia

Despite all the progress in the development of the recognition of autism, the second edition of the DSM, which was released in 1968, continued to recognize autism under schizophrenia, childhood type and was characterized by "atypical withdrawal behavior", "failure to develop identity separate from the mother's", and "general unevenness, gross immaturity, and inadequacy in development" highlighting the influence of psychoanalytic ideas on the term autism[28].

In 1978, Rutter[29] modified the original criteria proposed by Kanner and suggested a conceptual definition of autism that included delayed and deviant social and language abilities, general developmental level, restricted interest, and repetitive behavior. Rutter’s definition was influential on the advent of the DSM-III[29]. Interestingly, Rutter’s definition separated restricted interest from repetitive behavior[29].

Following extensive research that accumulated after the DSM-II era, the DSM-III finally distinguished between childhood-onset schizophrenia and autism in 1980[17]. DSM-III described autism as a pervasive developmental disorder (PDD) and defined three essential features of autism: A lack of interest in people; severe impairments in communication; and bizarre responses to the environment. These features develop in the first 30 months of life[17]. The definition of infantile autism was monothetic and required all criteria to be met. This produced some limitations in the sensitivity, and the term infantile provided a less developmentally focused approach. With further studies, autism was considered a heterogeneous disorder with significant presentation diversity, comorbidities, and severity[2].

Refining the identity of autism

The DSM-III-R moved from the term “infantile autism” to “autistic disorder” as the name for the condition to reflect a more flexible and developmentally oriented approach. In addition, the DSM provided a dimensional approach to reflect this heterogeneity and provided a polythetic set of 16 criteria. To simplify the complexity stemming from the heterogeneity in symptom range and severity, a model was created to organize these symptoms based on clinical judgment of these symptoms’ similarities into three major domains: (1) Qualitative impairments in reciprocal social interaction; (2) Impairment in communication; and (3) Restricted interest/resistance to change and repetitive movements. This approach was later criticized for being arbitrary and not supported by an empirical factor structure[30,31]. The diagnosis required the presence of eight positive criteria, two from the social domain and at least one from each of the other two domains. They also changed the onset requirement from 30 months to 36 months[31]. Table 1 summarizes DSM-III-R diagnostic criteria for autism disorder. PDD-not otherwise specified was used for patients who did not fulfill all the criteria for autism disorder.

Table 1 Summary of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-III-R diagnostic criteria for autism disorder.
Qualitative impairments in reciprocal social interaction
Impairment in communication
Restricted interest/resistance to change and repetitive movements
Lack of awareness No communicationStereotypies
Seeking comfortAbnormal nonverbalParts of objects
ImitationImaginative activityEnvironmental change
Social playProduction-speechInsistence-routine
Peer friendshipForm/content-speechRestricted interests
Conversation

When DSM-IV, initially released in 1994, was revised in 2000 to form DSM-IV-TR, the language was changed to recognize autism as a spectrum[32,33]. In addition, different assessment tools were used for research in preparation for the DSM-IV. They used factor analysis, a multivariate statistical method used to evaluate relationships among a set of observed variables, to reduce the heterogeneity of symptoms. Several models produced a different number of dimensions. The first was the traditional three-factor group. The second group was a two-factor (social/communication and restricted behaviors). Finally, a five-factor model (social, communication, restricted interests, stereotyped mannerisms, and adherence to routine) was established.

The basis for assigning many of these ASD symptoms under suggested domains in both DSM-IV and DSM-IV-TR was cluster analysis[34]. Cluster analysis can minimize the effect on homogeneity by grouping the sample items based on similarities and differences. This method can produce several benefits, such as developing diagnostic criteria, explaining heterogeneous outcomes, and tailoring treatment to subgroups. Despite the importance of cluster analysis, especially in developing domains related to ASD, cluster analysis cannot differentiate between core features of the illness and common comorbidities with similar presenting symptoms[35].

DSM-IV and beyond

To avoid conflicts with international collaboration and studies, the DSM-IV followed the steps of the International Classification of Diseases-10. It continued to use the traditional three-category or dimensions model, with a final set of criteria that needed to be more numerous and detailed. In addition, it followed the International Classification of Diseases-10 and listed five disorders with distinct features, including Asperger’s disorder, childhood disintegrative disorder, and Rett syndrome in addition to ASD and PDD not otherwise specified[32]. Both DSM-IV and DSM-IV-TR require the delay or abnormal function in at least one of the following areas, with onset to age 3 years: (1) Social interaction; (2) Language as used in social communication; and (3) Symbolic or imaginative play. Table 2 summarizes DSM-IV and DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for ASD.

Table 2 Summary of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV and Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder.
Qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by two of the following
Qualitative impairment in communication as manifested by at least one of the following
Restricted repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interest, and activities, as manifested by at least one of the following
Marked impairment in the use of multiple nonverbal behaviors such as eye-to-eye gaze, facial expression, body posture, and gestures that regulate social interactionDelay in, or total lack of, the development of spoken language (not accompanied by an attempt to compensate through alternative modes of communication such as gestures or mime)Encompassing preoccupation with one or more stereotyped and restricted patterns of interest that are abnormal either in intensity or focus
Failure to develop peer relationships appropriate to the developmental level (e.g., by a lack of showing, bringing, or pointing out the object of interest)In individuals with adequate speech, marked impairment in the ability to initiate or sustain a conversation with othersInflexible adherence to specific, nonfunctional routines or rituals
Lack of social or emotional reciprocityStereotyped and repetitive use of language or idiosyncratic languageStereotyped and repetitive motor manners (e.g., hand or finger flapping or twisting or complex whole-body movement)
Lack of varied, spontaneous make-believe play or social imitative play appropriate to the developmental levelPersistent preoccupation with parts of objects

In 2013, DSM-5, and later DSM-5-TR, adopted the concept of ASD, moving from a multicategorical diagnostic system into a single diagnosis, collapsing autism, Asperger Syndrome, Rett Syndrome, and childhood disintegrative disorder into a single diagnosis, in addition to unspecified neurodevelopmental disorder[1,18]. This approach was reported to have adequate sensitivity and specificity[36]. They also reduced the previous three-domain symptom model into a two-domain symptom model by combining the communication and social symptom categories into a single social-communication domain, keeping the RRB domain separate. This change was driven by factor analysis results that supported combining social and communication into a single domain and recognizing that categorizing behaviors as social or communicative may be an arbitrary division. The DSM-5 and DSM-5-TR provided a core symptom domain for the severity level of support needed for individual functioning and specifiers that offer descriptions of common co-occurring non-ASD impairment, such as intellectual impairment, language deficits, and medical and psychiatric disorders[37-39].

The DSM-5-TR diagnostic criteria for ASD recognize several psychopathological deficits across two domains of function. The first domain is social communication and interaction, where deficits exist across several contexts in social-emotional reciprocity, non-verbal communications, and developing, maintaining, and understanding relationships. The second domain is RRB patterns, interests, or activities. Within this domain, four areas of deficits are recognized. The diagnosis requires the presence of deficits in at least two of the four areas: (1) Stereotyped or repetitive motor movements, use of objects, or speech; (2) Insistence on sameness, inflexible adherence to routines, or ritualized patterns of verbal or nonverbal behavior; (3) Highly restricted, fixated interests that are abnormal in intensity or focus; and (4) Hyper-reactivity or hyporeactivity to sensory input or unusual interest in sensory aspects of the environment[40]. Table 3 presents DSM-5-TR criteria for ASD.

Table 3 Summary of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5 and Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5-TR diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder.
Persistent deficits in social communication and social interaction across multiple contexts, as manifested by the current or by history
Restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, or activities, as manifested by at least two of the following
(1) Deficits in nonverbal communicative behaviors used for social interaction, ranging from poorly integrated verbal and nonverbal communication to abnormalities in eye-to-eye contact and body language, or deficits in understanding and use of gesture, to total lack of facial expression and nonverbal communication(1) Stereotyped or repetitive motor movements, use of objects, or speech (e.g., simple motor stereotypies, lining up toys, or flipping objects, echolalia, idiosyncratic phrases)
(2) Deficits in developing, maintaining, and understanding relationships, ranging, for example, from difficulties in behavior to suit various social contexts, difficulties in sharing imaginative play or making friends, to the absence of interest in peers
(2) Insistence on sameness, inflexible adherence to routine, ritualized patterns, or verbal or nonverbal behavior (e.g., extreme distress at small changes, difficulties with transitions, rigid thinking patterns, greeting rituals, need to take the same route, or eating daily)
(3) Deficits in developing, maintaining, and understanding relationships, ranging, for example, from difficulties adjusting behavior to suit various social contexts, to difficulties in sharing imaginative play or making friends, to an absence of interest in peers(3) Highly restricted, fixated interests that are abnormal in intensity or focus (e.g., strong attachment to preoccupation with unusual objects, excessively circumscribed or perseverative interests)
(4) Hyper-reactivity or hyporeactivity to sensory input or unusual interest in sensory aspects of the environment (e.g., apparent indifference to pain/temperature, adverse response to specific sounds or textures, excessive smelling or touching of objects, visual fascination with lights or movement)
CATATONIA AND ASD OVERLAP

Existing nosological structures do not adequately distinguish RRB of neurodevelopmental disorders from motor and behavioral features of catatonia, and historical structures conflated both with schizophrenia. Cluster analysis, while shaping domains of ASD, struggles to separate intrinsic traits from co-occurring conditions like catatonia, setting the stage for reevaluating their relationship. This section will discuss the overlap between catatonia in phenomenology, neurobiology, and the response to similar treatment modalities.

Catatonia phenomenology

Catatonia was also previously linked to schizophrenia, and this position continued to dominate the field from Kraepelin until DSM-5[18,41-43]. In contrast to the older views of catatonia being related to schizophrenia, catatonia is commonly observed in the course of various psychiatric disorders, particularly bipolar disorder and depression[43]. Catatonia is also seen among several degenerative and demyelinating neurological diseases, such as Parkinson’s disease[44], multiple sclerosis, and epilepsy[45]. Oldham[11] suggested that the combined rate of catatonia among patients with structural abnormalities in the central nervous system and developmental disorders is nearly 11.6%. In addition, catatonia is frequently observed among patients with neurodevelopmental disorders such as ASD, with prevalence estimated to be between 4%-17%[12]. Catatonia is also described in the course of several medical conditions. As a result, both DSM-5 and DSM-5-TR recognized catatonia as a specifier that occurs with several psychiatric disorders and medical conditions[1,18].

While catatonia is commonly recognized as a psychomotor syndrome[10,46], a more comprehensive view recognizes catatonia as a syndrome with prominent psychomotor and cognitive manifestations. This article focused on the current catatonia nosology rather than its historical and affective manifestations[9]. Current DSM-5-TR criteria focus on motor and behavioral presentations with features such as mutism, posturing, stupor, grimacing, mannerism, stereotypies, echolalia, echopraxia, negativism, excitement, waxy flexibility, and catalepsy[34]. Table 4 summarizes the current DSM-5-TR diagnostic criteria for catatonia.

Table 4 Symptoms of catatonia based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5-TR.
Catatonia symptoms
Definitions
MutismNo or very little verbal response (excluding known aphasia)
PosturingSpontaneous and active maintenance of a posture against gravity
StuporNo psychomotor activity; not actively relating to the environment. An extreme manifestation of the previous phenomena
AgitationEmotionally restless, not as a result of external stimuli
Waxy flexibilitySlight, even resistance to positioning by the examiner
CatalepsyPassive induction of a posture held against gravity
EcholaliaMimicking another’s speech
EchopraxiaMimicking another’s movements
NegativismOpposition or no response to instruction or external stimuli. Negativism can be a complex behavior
StereotypiesRepetitive, abnormally frequent, non-goal-directed movements
MannerismsOdd, circumstantial caricature of normal actions
GrimacingDisplaying contorted facial expressions

Other clinical catatonia scales have been developed for diagnosis and monitoring[47]. These scales are the Bush Francis Catatonia Rating Scale[48], Modified Rogers Scale[49], Rogers Catatonia Scale[50], Northoff Catatonia Rating Scale[10], Kanner Scale[51], Brauning Catatonia Rating Scale[52], and the Pediatric Catatonia Rating Scale[53]. Table 5 summarizes all catatonia presentations in the Bush Francis Catatonia Rating Scale, Modified Rogers Scale, Rogers Catatonia Scale, Northoff Catatonia Rating Scale, Kanner, Brauning Catatonia Rating Scale, and Pediatric Catatonia Rating Scale with the overlap between these catatonia presentations and Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5-TR diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder.

Table 5 List of catatonia symptoms reported by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder-5-TR and catatonia clinical scales (Bush Francis Catatonia Rating Scale, Modified Rogers Scale, Rogers Catatonia Scale, Northoff Catatonia Rating Scale, Kanner Scale, Brauning Catatonia Rating Scale, and Pediatric Catatonia Rating Scale), with the overlap between these catatonia presentations and Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5-TR diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder.
Overlapping features of catatonia on the DSM-5-TR and the (BFCRS, MRS-C, RCS, NCRS, Kanner, BCRS, and PCRS) scales
Corresponding similar or related symptoms in ASD based on the DSM-5-TR criteria and other features reported in ASD
Echolalia/echopraxia (BFCRS, MRS-C, RCS, NCRS, Kanner, BCRS, and PCRS)Reported
Stereotypy (BFCRS, MRS-C, RCS, NCRS, Kanner, BCRS, and PCRS)Reported
Mannerism (BFCRS, MRS-C, RCS, NCRS, Kanner, BCRS, and PCRS)Reported
Excitement (BFCRS, MRS-C, RCS, NCRS, Kanner, BCRS, and PCRS)Reported
Grimacing (BFCRS, NCRS, Kanner, BCRS, and PCRS)Reported (facial mannerisms/stereotypies)
Verbigeration (BFCRS, MRS-C, RCS, NCRS, Kanner, BCRS, and PCRS)Reported
Impulsivity (BFCRS, NCRS, Kanner, BCRS, and PCRS)Reported
Combativeness (BFCRS, Kanner)Reported
Perseveration (BFCRS, NCRS, Kanner)Reported
Abnormal speech (MRC-S, RCS, NCRS)Reported
Rituals (MRC-S, NCRS)Reported
Compulsive behaviors (NCRS, PCRS)Reported
Compulsive-like speech (NCRS)Reported

Examining all the various presentations in the DSM-5-TR catatonia criteria and the various clinical scales show a noticeable degree of overlap in symptoms. In addition, multiple presentations of similar or identical catatonic phenomena are captured under different terms, such as catalepsy and posturing, mitgehen, mitmachen and magnetism, withdrawal and negativism, impulsivity, excitement, and agitation. Another challenge in recognizing and diagnosing catatonia is the lack of consistency in describing presentations of mannerisms and stereotypies and the various complex manifestations of these phenomena, such as grimacing, increased blinking, compulsive behaviors, compulsive speech, and verbigerations.

Catatonia and ASD phenomenological overlap

Several catatonia symptoms are also described as part of the core features of ASD, especially among the RRB and action domains of ASD. Examples of symptoms that would fall under this category include stereotypies of speech (echolalia, idiosyncratic language, perseverative language, or repetitive vocalizations such as grunting or humming) as well as stereotypical motor movements, which range from repetitive movements of hands (clapping, flapping, etc.) to complex whole-body movements, intense body tensing, facial grimacing, and other perseverative actions. These symptoms and behaviors do not just resemble symptoms described in catatonia but rather are clinically identical (Table 5). Mahgoub et al[54] discussed the inconsistencies in the psychiatric literature regarding the descriptions of mannerisms and stereotypies. They highlighted the varying definitions provided in different editions of the DSM for these phenomena, which are addressed under various conditions such as catatonia, ASD, and stereotypical movement disorders. The authors proposed that these activities are not limited to simple motor movements; they can also encompass complex behaviors, including repetitive and fixated eating and drinking. Additionally, they noted behaviors typically observed in patients with ASD, such as repetitive self-harm and mutilation, can be considered as stereotypies.

Mahgoub et al[54] suggested that evaluating complex behaviors within their appropriate context, including the patient’s typical or prior behavioral norms, could help link these behaviors to underlying pathological processes. This approach may provide clinicians with more effective methods for recognizing complex or unique behavioral presentations of catatonia similar to polydipsia and polyphagia.

Finally, they proposed distinct definitions: Mannerisms as non-contextual, fixated peculiarities of behavior; and stereotypies as non-contextual, repetitive activities. They argued that incorporating context and describing patterns can enhance understanding of these phenomena, particularly when describing complex activities observed in ASD and catatonia.

Other catatonic features include some other presentations that are seen in both ASD and catatonia, such as agitation, excitement, and impulsivity (Figure 1). The criterion in the diagnosis of ASD is deficits in social communication, and while it has been posited that negativism, withdrawal, and mutism of catatonia resemble these social deficits, the typical onset and acuity of mutism in catatonia is different from baseline chronic mutism that can be seen in ASD. In addition, deficits in social-emotional reciprocity do not provide an adequate parallel to negativism due to the differences inherent in the early onset and lifelong nature of ASD compared to the more acute changes often seen in catatonia.

Figure 1
Figure 1 Overlap of autism spectrum disorder restricted repetitive behaviors and catatonia symptoms. ASD: Autism spectrum disorder.

The degree of overlap between many of the described features of the restrictive repetitive behaviors domain and several other presentations described in ASD, with presentations described in catatonia, has been described in the psychiatric literature as posing a significant diagnostic challenge[55]. Such similarities raise the question of their independence and whether both disorders are separate.

Neurobiological overlap of catatonia and ASD

Despite continual advancement, research regarding the neuroscience of disorders like catatonia and ASD is in its infancy; common trends can be noted across the disparate literature. Through empirical study, it has been proposed and supported that GABA-A hypofunction, dopamine hypoactivity at the D2 receptor, and glutamate hypoactivity at the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor have the potential to predispose, initiate, and maintain catatonia and the related neuroleptic malignant syndrome[56]. GABA-A agonists and NMDA antagonists may ameliorate catatonia, while it can be initiated or worsened with D2-blocking antipsychotics.

At the circuit level, imaging studies in hypokinetic catatonia show hyperactivity of premotor areas related to cortico-cortical or cortico-basal ganglia loops[57-59]. Similar motor pathways are implicated in analogous research on stereotypies in ASD[60]. In addition to imaging studies suggesting the importance of specific sites like the premotor areas, anterior cingulate cortex, and orbitofrontal cortex in catatonia, emerging genetic research is showing how intracellular scaffolding and support proteins such as SHANK3 and MLC1 represent significant predisposing factors to both sporadic and periodic catatonia, respectively[61,62]. Interestingly, these same genes are implicated in genetic syndromes in which ASD is a common feature[63,64]. Furthermore, multiple genetic syndromes associated with autism feature mutations in genes related to diverse synapse functioning, such as neuroligin 3, neuroligin 4, various synapsis, and notably subunits of the GABA-A and NMDA receptors[65].

Dhossche et al[66] studied three subjects with SHANK3 gene mutations or intragenic deletion and followed them for the emergence of psychomotor phenomena. All 3 patients showed various combinations of catatonic features during their lives, including posturing, psychomotor slowdown, prompt responses, pseudocompulsion, negativism, stereotypies, mannerisms, mutism, echolalia, echopraxia, ambitendency, and verbigeration. Their study supported the link between SHANK3 mutations and childhood catatonia, especially among children with neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ASD.

The common genetic vulnerabilities reinforce an intimate relationship between ASD and catatonia. However, aberrations in these same genes and pathways are also implicated in other neuropsychiatric disorders, such as intellectual disability and epilepsy[67].

While emerging genetic research highlights shared vulnerabilities (e.g., SHANK3, MLC1) and neurobiological overlaps (e.g., GABA-A and NMDA receptor dysfunction), these findings remain preliminary. Robust empirical studies, such as genome-wide association studies or functional imaging trials, are needed to confirm these links and distinguish them from broader neuropsychiatric patterns seen in epilepsy or intellectual disability.

Shared response to similar modalities of treatment

Catatonia has been increasingly recognized in the last two decades among patients with ASD, with the focus on treating symptoms of motor and verbal mannerisms, echolalia, ritualistic behavior, obsessive preoccupation, excitement, aggression, impulsivity, and stereotypical self-harm behavior. The positive response to lorazepam was reported in several studies[68-73], and the role of ECT in treating catatonia in patients with ASD was also discussed extensively[15,74-77].

Some presentations in patients with ASD, such as repeated self-injurious behaviors, which are not classically characterized as related to catatonia, respond adequately to ECT[14,78,79]. Given its non-contextual repeated activity, repeated self-injurious behavior can be categorized as a complex form of catatonic stereotypy. This may explain the excellent response to ECT.

With the significant overlap between catatonia and ASD diagnostic criteria, several authors attempted to address these diagnostic challenges, and some approaches were suggested. One approach suggested that catatonia should be suspected when there is a change in the type or pattern of premorbid functioning or prior established symptoms[80]. Nordgaard et al[81] supported this approach. They emphasized a delineation between state and trait disorders, whereby one condition may be chronically present and inform all aspects of life, while another may periodically present with circumscribed episodes. Furthermore, the author and others argue compellingly in favor of hierarchical psychiatric nosologies; the compartmentalization of discrete symptom clusters is reduced to understand the patient experience more holistically[80,82].

Other authors suggested using the term autistic catatonia, which describes patients with ASD who present with prominent negativism and psychomotor retardation for over 1 month that involves freezing when carrying out actions, very slow voluntary movements, stopping the course of movements, and requiring prompting to complete actions[83]. This concept was modified later to suggest that odd gait, odd stiff posture, mutism, impulsive actions, incontinence, and excitement are more likely to suggest catatonia when observed among patients with ASD[84]. Most of the studies that showed a response to lorazepam or ECT recognized catatonia in ASD patients by observing changes in prior patterns and course of illness. Such an approach leaves a remaining question about patients with ASD who have a stable course and pattern of overlapping symptoms that might still be framed as catatonia and respond to lorazepam and ECT.

The case for distinguishing catatonia from overlapping features of ASD lacks strong empirical support, particularly in neurobiology, where data are suggestive but not definitive.

Evidence for overlap

ASD and catatonia share extensive phenomenological, neurobiological, and therapeutic parallels. RRB, like echolalia and stereotypies, are clinically identical to catatonic features, and both respond to similar interventions. Neurobiological links, though preliminary, reinforce this connection. Hence, it is essential to provide more details about the concept of comorbidity and the standard used in the DSM to separate overlapping conditions.

COMORBID OR OVERLAPPING DISORDERS?
The concept of comorbidity

Feinstein[85] was the first to discuss the concept of comorbidity. He defined comorbidity as a distinct additional clinical entity that has existed or may occur during the clinical course of a patient with the index disease under study. Feinstein’s concept was based on the notion of distinct additional clinical entities and most importantly that a medical condition must have either known etiology or circumscribed pathology to qualify as a distinct additional clinical entity[85,86]. With the development of DSM-III, the concept of comorbidity was also used to describe the co-occurrence of two or more mental disorders[87]. However, applying Feinstein’s definition in psychiatry was challenging compared to other branches of medicine, as most psychiatric disorders do not have a well-circumscribed psychopathology or biomarkers that can be equivalent to the pathology in Feinstein’s model. In addition, many psychiatric disorders are not demarcated from each other due to symptom overlap. Hence, the notion of distinct clinical entities is lacking in psychiatry[75,88].

The application of the concept of comorbidity in psychiatry resulted in three forms of outcomes. The first is true comorbidity, which matches Feinstein’s requirement for distinct additional clinical entities. The second is artificial comorbidity, commonly seen in the DSM due to the split and categorical system applied, where overlapping symptoms might suggest the presence of two diagnoses. Finally, spurious comorbidity can be detected, as in the examples of autistic disorder and Asperger’s disorder in DSM-IV[89].

Several approaches have been utilized to improve diagnostic validity and reduce artificial and spurious comorbidity. The Robins and Guze[90] diagnostic (construct) validity served as the basis for DSM-III diagnoses in clearly delineating several disorders as clinically distinct criteria. These criteria are: (1) Clinical description: Identification and description of the syndrome, either by clinical intuition or by cluster analysis. Cluster analysis was used extensively to validate clinical criteria and descriptions. However, the criteria used for cluster analysis should be the best discriminators. As discussed above, cluster analysis needs to be able to differentiate between core features and common comorbidities; (2) Exclusion of other disorders: This is important to show boundaries or points of rarity between related syndromes. While relevant, this point must be achieved to differentiate between many psychiatric syndromes; (3) Follow-up study: To assess the presence of distinct courses over time and therapeutic trials showing distinct treatment responses; (4) Family study: To assess the genetics or hereditability aspects of the illness; and (5) Laboratory study: This identifies neurobiological or pathological aspects of the disorder being assessed, including psychological, genetic, molecular, histological, and radiological factors[90,91].

Challenges in separation

As we attempt to apply these criteria to ASD in relationship to catatonia, it becomes evident that many of the suggested criteria for diagnostic validity are not met, as the clinical description shows significant overlap in symptoms, especially in the domain of RRB such as the various presentations of mannerisms, stereotypies, echolalia, perseveration, excitement, and various obsessive behaviors. While it is not uncommon to have overlapping presentations between various psychiatric disorders, these symptoms are identical in both disorders to the degree of making the second criterion of exclusion of other disorders, catatonia in this case, almost impossible. Many of the RRB are historically viewed as intrinsic to ASD due to their early onset and consistency, but their striking similarity to catatonic symptoms and response to treatments like ECT challenge this assumption. Follow-up studies, especially the shared response to similar treatment modalities, do not support appropriate separation, especially when taken with the rest of the factors studied and similarly the extensive neurobiological overlap. Although this might be less specific, the research in this area is in its infancy. Family studies in this area appear to be lacking. Table 6 summarizes the results of applying the Robins and Guze[90] validity criteria to ASD in relation to catatonia.

Table 6 Applying Robins and Guze validity criteria to autism spectrum disorder in relation to catatonia[90].
Robins and Guze factors[90]
Criteria satisfaction
Clinical descriptionSeveral identical features between catatonia and ASD
Exclusion of other disordersIt is challenging to exclude and differentiate both syndromes from each other based on criteria
Follow up studiesGood response to similar modalities of treatment, such as lorazepam and ECT; ASD-related catatonia may be more chronic than catatonia associated with mood, psychotic, and systemic medical disorders
Laboratory studiesThis area is in its infancy. However, a significant report shows an overlap in neurobiology
Family studiesNo data available
Counterarguments

Advocates of RRB as core ASD features emphasize their consistent emergence in infancy, distinguishing them from the typically acute or episodic onset of catatonia. Yet chronic catatonia, though understudied, is documented in developmental disorders[12], and its occurrence in ASD may obscure these onset differences. They contend that functional distinctions, such as the adaptive role of RRB vs the purposelessness of catatonia, could reflect severity rather than separate etiologies, with severe RRB mirroring catatonia due to shared underlying mechanisms. Although neurobiological overlaps (e.g., SHANK3 mutations, GABA-A dysfunction) lack specificity, their significance grows when aligned with clinical and therapeutic similarities, and treatment response variability might arise from phenotypic diversity rather than distinct conditions. These counterarguments underscore the need for robust empirical research, including longitudinal studies tracking RRB and catatonia onset and randomized trials of catatonia-specific treatments, to unravel this complexity.

IMPLICATIONS AND LIMITATIONS
Feasibility and safety of clinical applications

Proposing catatonia treatments like lorazepam and ECT for ASD raises questions of feasibility and safety. While small case studies report efficacy, large-scale clinical trials are absent, and risks (e.g., sedation with lorazepam, cognitive effects of ECT) remain unquantified in ASD populations. Ethical considerations, particularly for pediatric patients, further complicate adoption. Randomized controlled trials are essential to assess efficacy, tolerability, and long-term outcomes.

Reclassification

If RRB were reframed as catatonic features, clinicians might prioritize benzodiazepines or ECT earlier, even for stable symptoms, reducing distress. Current approaches flag catatonia only with symptom changes, leaving chronic cases unaddressed.

Limitations and future recommendations

While the extensive overlap between ASD and catatonia creates a compelling argument and catalyst for paradigm evolution in neuropsychiatry, this potential assignment remains provisional. It requires validation and more support based on genomic, larger-scale imaging correlates and rigorous electrophysiological studies. Several reports describe the efficacy of options for treating catatonia, like lorazepam and ECT. However, larger studies and protocols for these populations are needed to establish the safety and effectiveness of these treatments for patients with ASD.

Future multimodal studies that combine functional genomics with advanced neuroimaging modalities to map shared neural circuit abnormalities are needed. In addition, multicenter randomized controlled trials to evaluate GABAergic agents under strict phenotypic stratification, incorporating biomarkers and response to biological treatments, should be considered for treatment response predictors.

CONCLUSION

There is considerable overlap in phenomenology, neurobiology, response to the same modalities of treatment, and increased predisposition and frequency of catatonia in patients with ASD. Evidence supporting the assignment of these overlapping presentations as core features of ASD is weak, and the distinct comorbidity of catatonia, and ASD seems to be a more compelling concept. Several approaches for distinguishing between the two clinical presentations focused on identifying a change in the severity or pattern of previous symptoms to suggest catatonia. While the authors agree with this approach, it leaves the assignment of cases with no changes in their chronic symptom pattern or severity without a clear answer. Answering this question significantly impacts clinical care and future studies, as it might provide treatment options for many of the RRB in ASD not previously considered for treatment because of their chronicity and conceptualization as an intrinsic feature of ASD. Future research should include randomized trials testing lorazepam and ECT in ASD patients with chronic RRB, alongside longitudinal studies to track symptom evolution and refine diagnostic boundaries. Reclassifying these symptoms could not only enhance diagnostic precision but also unlock targeted interventions, improving the quality of life for a significant subset of ASD individuals.

Footnotes

Provenance and peer review: Invited article; Externally peer reviewed.

Peer-review model: Single blind

Specialty type: Psychiatry

Country of origin: United States

Peer-review report’s classification

Scientific Quality: Grade B, Grade B, Grade B, Grade C

Novelty: Grade B, Grade C, Grade C, Grade C

Creativity or Innovation: Grade B, Grade B, Grade C, Grade C

Scientific Significance: Grade B, Grade C, Grade C, Grade D

P-Reviewer: Nwabo Kamdje AH; Yan J S-Editor: Fan M L-Editor: Filipodia P-Editor: Li X

References
1.  American Psychiatric Association  5th ed. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 2022.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]
2.  Mannion A, Leader G. Comorbidity in autism spectrum disorder: A literature review. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2013;7:1595-1616.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 198]  [Cited by in RCA: 124]  [Article Influence: 10.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
3.  Anderson JC, Williams S, McGee R, Silva PA. DSM-III disorders in preadolescent children. Prevalence in a large sample from the general population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44:69-76.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1015]  [Cited by in RCA: 776]  [Article Influence: 20.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
4.  Lewinsohn PM, Striegel-Moore RH, Seeley JR. Epidemiology and natural course of eating disorders in young women from adolescence to young adulthood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39:1284-1292.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 295]  [Cited by in RCA: 276]  [Article Influence: 11.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
5.  Ghaziuddin M, Weidmer-Mikhail E, Ghaziuddin N. Comorbidity of Asperger syndrome: a preliminary report. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1998;42 (Pt 4):279-283.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 208]  [Cited by in RCA: 136]  [Article Influence: 5.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
6.  van Steensel FJ, Bögels SM, Perrin S. Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14:302-317.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 836]  [Cited by in RCA: 810]  [Article Influence: 62.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
7.  Tuchman R, Rapin I. Epilepsy in autism. Lancet Neurol. 2002;1:352-358.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 493]  [Cited by in RCA: 428]  [Article Influence: 18.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
8.  Mannion A, Leader G. An analysis of the predictors of comorbid psychopathology, gastrointestinal symptoms and epilepsy in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2013;7:1663-1671.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]
9.  Srivastava AK, Schwartz CE. Intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorders: causal genes and molecular mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;46 Pt 2:161-174.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 133]  [Cited by in RCA: 145]  [Article Influence: 13.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
10.  Northoff G, Koch A, Wenke J, Eckert J, Böker H, Pflug B, Bogerts B. Catatonia as a psychomotor syndrome: a rating scale and extrapyramidal motor symptoms. Mov Disord. 1999;14:404-416.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]
11.  Oldham MA. The Probability That Catatonia in the Hospital has a Medical Cause and the Relative Proportions of Its Causes: A Systematic Review. Psychosomatics. 2018;59:333-340.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 26]  [Cited by in RCA: 34]  [Article Influence: 4.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
12.  Dhossche DM. Decalogue of catatonia in autism spectrum disorders. Front Psychiatry. 2014;5:157.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 26]  [Cited by in RCA: 18]  [Article Influence: 1.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
13.  Mormando C, Mikoluk C, Garman JC, Rapp M, Murray M, Francis A. Electroconvulsive Therapy for Autism-related Stereotyped Emesis After Chiari Malformation Type 1 Craniectomy. J ECT. 2021;37:e7-e8.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in RCA: 4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
14.  Wachtel LE, Contrucci-Kuhn SA, Griffin M, Thompson A, Dhossche DM, Reti IM. ECT for self-injury in an autistic boy. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;18:458-463.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 45]  [Cited by in RCA: 42]  [Article Influence: 2.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
15.  Burns NK, Grissett K, Macaluso M, Raza M, Gracious B. Excited Catatonia in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Case Series. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:674335.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 4]  [Cited by in RCA: 5]  [Article Influence: 1.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
16.  Braun V, Clarke V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual Res Psychol. 2006;3:77-101.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 64231]  [Cited by in RCA: 64920]  [Article Influence: 3416.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
17.  Kendell RE. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd ed. AJP. 1980;137:1630-1631.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]
18.  American Psychiatric Association  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5™, 5th ed. 2013.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 66101]  [Cited by in RCA: 56903]  [Article Influence: 3556.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (3)]
19.  American Psychiatric Association  Dementia praecox or the group of schizophrenias. [cited March 18, 2025]. Available from: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1951-03305-000.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
20.  Evans B. How autism became autism: The radical transformation of a central concept of child development in Britain. Hist Human Sci. 2013;26:3-31.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 114]  [Cited by in RCA: 68]  [Article Influence: 5.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
21.  Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child. 1943;2:217-250.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
22.  Kanner L. Infantile autism and the schizophrenias. Behav Sci. 1965;10:412-420.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 58]  [Cited by in RCA: 60]  [Article Influence: 1.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
23.  Mahler MS. On Child Psychosis and Schizophrenia. Psychoanal Study Child. 1952;7:286-305.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 257]  [Cited by in RCA: 155]  [Article Influence: 19.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
24.  Rimland B  Infantile autism: The syndrome and its implications for a neural theory of behavior. Edelson SM, editor. Jessica Kingsley Publishers.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
25.  Neumärker KJ. Classification matters for catatonia and autism in children. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2006;72:3-19.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 5]  [Cited by in RCA: 4]  [Article Influence: 0.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
26.  Neumärker KJ. Leonhard and the classification of psychomotor psychoses in childhood and adolescence. Psychopathology. 1990;23:243-252.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 9]  [Cited by in RCA: 8]  [Article Influence: 0.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
27.  Petty LK, Ornitz EM, Michelman JD, Zimmerman EG. Autistic children who become schizophrenic. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1984;41:129-135.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 144]  [Cited by in RCA: 146]  [Article Influence: 3.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
28.  American Psychiatric Association  Committee on Nomenclature and Statistics. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 2nd. Washington: 1968.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
29.  Rutter M. Diagnosis and definition of childhood autism. J Autism Child Schizophr. 1978;8:139-161.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 438]  [Cited by in RCA: 300]  [Article Influence: 6.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
30.  Masi A, DeMayo MM, Glozier N, Guastella AJ. An Overview of Autism Spectrum Disorder, Heterogeneity and Treatment Options. Neurosci Bull. 2017;33:183-193.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 667]  [Cited by in RCA: 483]  [Article Influence: 60.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
31.  American Psychiatric Association  Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders; revised (DSM-III-R). Washington DC. 1987.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
32.  American Psychiatric Association  Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Association, 1994: 70-71.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
33.  American Psychiatric Association  Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Association, 2000: 70-71.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
34.  Guthrie W, Swineford LB, Wetherby AM, Lord C. Comparison of DSM-IV and DSM-5 factor structure models for toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52:797-805.e2.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 32]  [Cited by in RCA: 33]  [Article Influence: 2.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
35.  Van den Berg MD, Oldehinkel AJ, Bouhuys AL, Brilman EI, Beekman AT, Ormel J. Depression in later life: three etiologically different subgroups. J Affect Disord. 2001;65:19-26.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 56]  [Cited by in RCA: 52]  [Article Influence: 2.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
36.  Huerta M, Bishop SL, Duncan A, Hus V, Lord C. Application of DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder to three samples of children with DSM-IV diagnoses of pervasive developmental disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169:1056-1064.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 207]  [Cited by in RCA: 161]  [Article Influence: 12.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
37.  Gotham K, Risi S, Pickles A, Lord C. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:613-627.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 777]  [Cited by in RCA: 801]  [Article Influence: 42.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
38.  Robertson JM, Tanguay PE, L'Ecuyer S, Sims A, Waltrip C. Domains of social communication handicap in autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38:738-745.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 65]  [Cited by in RCA: 67]  [Article Influence: 2.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
39.  Zuddas A. Autism assessment tools in the transition from DSM-IV to DSM-5. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;22:325-327.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 3]  [Cited by in RCA: 3]  [Article Influence: 0.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
40.  Rosen NE, Lord C, Volkmar FR. The Diagnosis of Autism: From Kanner to DSM-III to DSM-5 and Beyond. J Autism Dev Disord. 2021;51:4253-4270.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 90]  [Cited by in RCA: 86]  [Article Influence: 21.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
41.  Kraepelin E  Psychiatrie : ein Lehrbuch für Studierende und Ärzte. Vol 2. 6th ed. Barth JA, editor. 1899.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
42.  Gazdag G, Takács R, Ungvari GS. Catatonia as a putative nosological entity: A historical sketch. World J Psychiatry. 2017;7:177-183.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in CrossRef: 12]  [Cited by in RCA: 11]  [Article Influence: 1.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
43.  Taylor MA, Abrams R. Catatonia. Prevalence and importance in the manic phase of manic-depressive illness. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1977;34:1223-1225.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 85]  [Cited by in RCA: 70]  [Article Influence: 1.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
44.  Poyraz BÇ, Aksoy Poyraz C, Yassa A, Arikan MK, Gündüz A, Kiziltan G. Recurrent Catatonia in Parkinson Disease. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2016;36:104-106.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 5]  [Cited by in RCA: 6]  [Article Influence: 0.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
45.  Jiang SM, Koepsell M, Patel B, Athanasiadi A. Recurrent Catatonia and Demyelinating Disorders. Cureus. 2023;15:e41656.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
46.  Rasmussen SA, Mazurek MF, Rosebush PI. Catatonia: Our current understanding of its diagnosis, treatment and pathophysiology. World J Psychiatry. 2016;6:391-398.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in CrossRef: 149]  [Cited by in RCA: 177]  [Article Influence: 19.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (9)]
47.  Sienaert P, Rooseleer J, De Fruyt J. Measuring catatonia: a systematic review of rating scales. J Affect Disord. 2011;135:1-9.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 94]  [Cited by in RCA: 100]  [Article Influence: 7.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
48.  Bush G, Fink M, Petrides G, Dowling F, Francis A. Catatonia. I. Rating scale and standardized examination. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1996;93:129-136.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 588]  [Cited by in RCA: 654]  [Article Influence: 22.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
49.  Lund CE, Mortimer AM, Rogers D, McKenna PJ. Motor, volitional and behavioural disorders in schizophrenia. 1: Assessment using the Modified Rogers Scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1991;158:323-327, 333.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 89]  [Cited by in RCA: 83]  [Article Influence: 2.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
50.  Starkstein SE, Petracca G, Tesón A, Chemerinski E, Merello M, Migliorelli R, Leiguarda R. Catatonia in depression: prevalence, clinical correlates, and validation of a scale. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1996;60:326-332.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 82]  [Cited by in RCA: 83]  [Article Influence: 2.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
51.  Carroll BT, Kirkhart R, Ahuja N, Soovere I, Lauterbach EC, Dhossche D, Talbert R. Katatonia: a new conceptual understanding of catatonia and a new rating scale. Psychiatry (Edgmont). 2008;5:42-50.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
52.  Bräunig P, Krüger S, Shugar G, Höffler J, Börner I. The catatonia rating scale I--development, reliability, and use. Compr Psychiatry. 2000;41:147-158.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 68]  [Cited by in RCA: 59]  [Article Influence: 2.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
53.  Benarous X, Consoli A, Raffin M, Bodeau N, Giannitelli M, Cohen D, Olliac B. Validation of the Pediatric Catatonia Rating Scale (PCRS). Schizophr Res. 2016;176:378-386.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 29]  [Cited by in RCA: 54]  [Article Influence: 6.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
54.  Mahgoub Y, Pathare A, Hamlin D, Kindt H, Francis A. Mannerisms and stereotypies in catatonia: beyond simple motor movements. Front Psychiatry. 2024;15:1435719.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Reference Citation Analysis (1)]
55.  Penland HR, Weder N, Tampi RR. The catatonic dilemma expanded. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2006;5:14.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 43]  [Cited by in RCA: 42]  [Article Influence: 2.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
56.  Carroll BT. The universal field hypothesis of catatonia and neuroleptic malignant syndrome. CNS Spectr. 2000;5:26-33.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 46]  [Cited by in RCA: 48]  [Article Influence: 1.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
57.  Walther S, Schäppi L, Federspiel A, Bohlhalter S, Wiest R, Strik W, Stegmayer K. Resting-State Hyperperfusion of the Supplementary Motor Area in Catatonia. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43:972-981.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 21]  [Cited by in RCA: 51]  [Article Influence: 6.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
58.  Viher PV, Stegmayer K, Federspiel A, Bohlhalter S, Wiest R, Walther S. Altered diffusion in motor white matter tracts in psychosis patients with catatonia. Schizophr Res. 2020;220:210-217.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 19]  [Cited by in RCA: 25]  [Article Influence: 5.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
59.  Péter Z, Oliphant ME, Fernandez TV. Motor Stereotypies: A Pathophysiological Review. Front Neurosci. 2017;11:171.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 34]  [Cited by in RCA: 35]  [Article Influence: 4.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
60.  Breckpot J, Vercruyssen M, Weyts E, Vandevoort S, D'Haenens G, Van Buggenhout G, Leempoels L, Brischoux-Boucher E, Van Maldergem L, Renieri A, Mencarelli MA, D'Angelo C, Mericq V, Hoffer MJ, Tauber M, Molinas C, Castiglioni C, Brison N, Vermeesch JR, Danckaerts M, Sienaert P, Devriendt K, Vogels A. Copy number variation analysis in adults with catatonia confirms haploinsufficiency of SHANK3 as a predisposing factor. Eur J Med Genet. 2016;59:436-443.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 17]  [Cited by in RCA: 22]  [Article Influence: 2.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
61.  Selch S, Strobel A, Haderlein J, Meyer J, Jacob CP, Schmitt A, Lesch KP, Reif A. MLC1 polymorphisms are specifically associated with periodic catatonia, a subgroup of chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:1211-1214.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 17]  [Cited by in RCA: 20]  [Article Influence: 1.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
62.  Pagani M, Bertero A, Liska A, Galbusera A, Sabbioni M, Barsotti N, Colenbier N, Marinazzo D, Scattoni ML, Pasqualetti M, Gozzi A. Deletion of Autism Risk Gene Shank3 Disrupts Prefrontal Connectivity. J Neurosci. 2019;39:5299-5310.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 55]  [Cited by in RCA: 70]  [Article Influence: 11.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
63.  Bonsi P, De Jaco A, Fasano L, Gubellini P. Postsynaptic autism spectrum disorder genes and synaptic dysfunction. Neurobiol Dis. 2022;162:105564.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 4]  [Cited by in RCA: 18]  [Article Influence: 4.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
64.  Rylaarsdam L, Guemez-Gamboa A. Genetic Causes and Modifiers of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front Cell Neurosci. 2019;13:385.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 376]  [Cited by in RCA: 276]  [Article Influence: 46.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (1)]
65.  Specchio N, Di Micco V, Trivisano M, Ferretti A, Curatolo P. The epilepsy-autism spectrum disorder phenotype in the era of molecular genetics and precision therapy. Epilepsia. 2022;63:6-21.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 4]  [Cited by in RCA: 15]  [Article Influence: 3.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
66.  Dhossche D, de Billy C, Laurent-Levinson C, Le Normand MT, Recasens C, Robel L, Philippe A. Early-onset catatonia associated with SHANK3 mutations: looking at the autism spectrum through the prism of psychomotor phenomena. Front Psychiatry. 2023;14:1186555.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
67.  Dhossche D. Brief report: catatonia in autistic disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1998;28:329-331.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 39]  [Cited by in RCA: 34]  [Article Influence: 1.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
68.  de Winter CF, van Dijk F, Verhoeven WM, Dhossche DM, Stolker JJ. [Autism and catatonia: successful treatment using lorazepam. A case study]. Tijdschr Psychiatr. 2007;49:257-261.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
69.  Kakooza-Mwesige A, Wachtel LE, Dhossche DM. Catatonia in autism: implications across the life span. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;17:327-335.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 65]  [Cited by in RCA: 58]  [Article Influence: 3.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
70.  Bozkurt H, Mukaddes NM. Catatonia in a child with autistic disorder. Turk J Pediatr. 2010;52:435-438.  [PubMed]  [DOI]
71.  Quilliam R, Quilliam S, Turnbull M, Parkinson S, Oligbu G. Catatonia as a presentation of autism in a child: a case report. AIMS Neurosci. 2020;7:327-332.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Full Text (PDF)]  [Cited by in Crossref: 2]  [Cited by in RCA: 4]  [Article Influence: 0.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
72.  De Stefano L, Palffy A, Ghaziuddin N. Catatonia in Preadolescent Children. J ECT. 2024;40:162-168.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
73.  Mishra BR, Biswas T, Chakraborty R, Bhoi R, Ranjan R, Pattojoshi A. Maintenance Electroconvulsive Therapy in Autism Spectrum Disorder With Comorbid Psychosis Presenting With Recurrent Pharmaco-Resistant Catatonia: A Case Report. J ECT. 2024;40:216-217.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
74.  Couse M, Makhinson M, Kelly P. Regulatory and Service System Challenges in Accessing Electroconvulsive Therapy for Catatonia in the Presence of Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2024;63:293-295.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
75.  Free M, Pathare A, Mahgoub Y. Unintended Weight Gain After Treatment of Catatonia With ECT in Autism: Case Report and Literature Review. J ECT. 2023;39:269-270.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
76.  Park SE, Grados M, Wachtel L, Kaji S. Use of Electroconvulsive Therapy in Autism. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2020;29:455-465.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 3]  [Cited by in RCA: 3]  [Article Influence: 0.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
77.  DʼAgati D, Chang AD, Wachtel LE, Reti IM. Treatment of Severe Self-Injurious Behavior in Autism Spectrum Disorder by Neuromodulation. J ECT. 2017;33:7-11.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 15]  [Cited by in RCA: 10]  [Article Influence: 1.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
78.  Adıgüzel Akman Ö, Kahraman Girgeç S, Çelik S, Çakır Kardeş V, Atasoy N. Maintenance Electroconvulsive Therapy for Agitation and Self Injurious Behaviors in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Turk Psikiyatri Derg. 2021;32:65-69.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1]  [Cited by in RCA: 1]  [Article Influence: 0.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
79.  Siegel M, Milligan B, Robbins D, Prentice G. Electroconvulsive therapy in an adolescent with autism and bipolar I disorder. J ECT. 2012;28:252-255.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 12]  [Cited by in RCA: 12]  [Article Influence: 0.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
80.  Ghaziuddin M, Quinlan P, Ghaziuddin N. Catatonia in autism: a distinct subtype? J Intellect Disabil Res. 2005;49:102-105.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 44]  [Cited by in RCA: 44]  [Article Influence: 2.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
81.  Nordgaard J, Madeira L, Shinn AK, Cermolacce M. Editorial: Psychiatric diagnoses: current state and methodological issues. Front Psychiatry. 2023;14:1194755.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in RCA: 1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
82.  Chaplin WF, John OP, Goldberg LR. Conceptions of states and traits: dimensional attributes with ideals as prototypes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54:541-557.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 11]  [Cited by in RCA: 31]  [Article Influence: 0.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
83.  Hare DJ, Malone C. Catatonia and autistic spectrum disorders. Autism. 2004;8:183-195.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 37]  [Cited by in RCA: 32]  [Article Influence: 3.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
84.  Dhossche DM, Shah A, Wing L. Blueprints for the assessment, treatment, and future study of catatonia in autism spectrum disorders. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2006;72:267-284.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 45]  [Cited by in RCA: 36]  [Article Influence: 1.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
85.  Feinstein AR. The pre-therapeutic classification of co-morbidity in chronic disease. J Chronic Dis. 1970;23:455-468.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 893]  [Cited by in RCA: 811]  [Article Influence: 81.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
86.  Nordgaard J, Nielsen KM, Rasmussen AR, Henriksen MG. Psychiatric comorbidity: a concept in need of a theory. Psychol Med. 2023;53:5902-5908.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
87.  Lilienfeld SO, Waldman ID, Israel AC. A critical examination of the use of the term and concept of comorbidity in psychopathology research. Clin Psychol-Sci Pr. 1994;1:71-83.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 73]  [Cited by in RCA: 73]  [Article Influence: 2.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
88.  Sharp C, Kalpakci A. If It Looks Like a Duck and Quacks Like a Duck: Evaluating the Validity of Borderline Personality Disorder in Adolescents. Scand J Child Adoles Psychiatry Psychol. 2014;3:49-62.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 3]  [Cited by in RCA: 3]  [Article Influence: 0.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
89.  First MB. Mutually exclusive versus co-occurring diagnostic categories: the challenge of diagnostic comorbidity. Psychopathology. 2005;38:206-210.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 61]  [Cited by in RCA: 60]  [Article Influence: 3.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
90.  Robins E, Guze SB. Establishment of diagnostic validity in psychiatric illness: its application to schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1970;126:983-987.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1260]  [Cited by in RCA: 1143]  [Article Influence: 20.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
91.  Kendell RE. Clinical validity. Psychol Med. 1989;19:45-55.  [RCA]  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Full Text]  [Cited by in Crossref: 226]  [Cited by in RCA: 225]  [Article Influence: 6.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]